不同类型情绪对认知抑制的影响
The Effects of Emotion on Cognitive Inhibition
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2013.35036, PDF, HTML,  被引量 下载: 3,892  浏览: 11,407 
作者: 贾丽萍*, 白学军, 王敬欣:天津师范大学心理与行为研究院
关键词: 情绪stroop认知抑制阻碍 Emotion; Stroop; Cognitive Inhibition; Disrupt
摘要: 本研究采用3(情绪类型:正性、负性、中性) × 3(任务类型:一致、不一致、无关)的被试内实验设计,考察了情绪对认知抑制的影响。以38名大学生为被试,从中国情绪图片库中选取正性、负性和中性图片各60张,并在stroop任务前呈现情绪图片,要求被试判断stroop任务中的汉字颜色。结果发现,任务类型主效应显著;情绪类型与任务类型的交互作用显著,与中性情绪条件相比,在正性情绪和负性情绪条件下,被试完成stroop任务更难,stroop效应更大,表明情绪对认知抑制的阻碍作用。
Abstract: To investigate the effects of emotion on cognition inhibition, a 3 (emotion type: positive, negative and neutral) by 3 (task type: congruent, incongruent and neutral) within subject design was conducted. Thirty eight university students participated in the experiment, emotional pictures (60 pictures for positive, negative and neutral scenes each) selected from the Chinese Affective Picture System (CAPS) were used as experi- ment materials to induce participants’ emotion and showed before the stroop task, in which the participants were asked to name the color of the word printed and ignore the word meaning. The results showed that the main effect of task type and the interaction of emotion type and task type were significant, further analyses revealed larger stroop effect for emotion condition than neutral condition. From the current results, we can conclude that emotion (positive and negative) can disrupt cognitive inhibition.
文章引用:贾丽萍, 白学军, 王敬欣 (2013). 不同类型情绪对认知抑制的影响. 心理学进展, 3(5), 233-237. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2013.35036

参考文献

[1] 白学军, 贾丽萍, 王敬欣(2013). 抑制范式下的情绪注意偏向. 心理科学进展, 5期, 785-791.
[2] Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psy- chological Review, 106, 529-550.
[3] Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 331-345.
[4] Bohner, G., Chaiken, S., & Hunyadi, P. (1994). The role of mood and message ambiguity in the interplay of heuristic and systematic proc- essing. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 207-221.
[5] Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Ef- fects of positive mood and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14, 416-421.
[6] Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognitive II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- science, 12, 1-47.
[7] Dolcos, F., McCarthy, G., (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 2072-2079.
[8] Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and though-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.
[9] Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., & Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 4115-4120.
[10] Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., & Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during Stroop task performance. Neuroimage, 49, 2662.
[11] Hartikainen, K. M., Ogawa, K. H., & Knight, R. T. (2000). Transient interference of right hemispheric function due to automatic emotional processing. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1576-1580.
[12] Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In T. Dalgleish, & M. Powers (Eds.), The handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 75-94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[13] Mathewson, K. J., Arnell, K. M., & Mansfield, C. A. (2008). Capturing and holding attention: The impact of emotional words in rapid serial visual presentation. Memory & Cognition, 36, 182-200.
[14] Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emo- tion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 654- 661.
[15] Nummenmaa, L., & Hyönä, J. (2009). Emotional scene content drives the saccade generation system reflexively. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 305, 305-323.
[16] Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and heuristics: The influence of happy and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1005-1023.
[17] Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 148-158.
[18] Pessoa, L., Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). Beyond brain regions: Network perspective of cognition-emotion interactions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 158.
[19] Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., & Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion-APA, 12, 192.
[20] Phillips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E., & Fraser, L. (2002). Positive mood and executive function: Evidence from stroop and fluency tasks. Emotion, 2, 21-22.
[21] Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2007). Selective visual attention to emotion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1082-1089.
[22] Yuan, J., Xu, S., Yang, J., Liu, Q., Chen, A., Zhu, L., & Li, H. (2011). Pleasant mood intensifies brain processing of cognitive control: ERP correlates. Biological Psychology, 87, 17-24.