视觉工作记忆内容引导情绪面孔的视觉搜索
Visual Working Memory Influence the Attentional Selection of Emotional Faces
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2014.41018, PDF, HTML, 下载: 3,189  浏览: 8,215  国家科技经费支持
作者: 范玲霞:西南大学心理学部,重庆;;Cody Ding:西南大学心理学部,重庆;密苏里大学圣路易斯分校教育心理学系,圣路易斯;郭仁露:温州大学心理与行为研究所,温州;杨 东:西南大学心理学部,重庆
关键词: 工作记忆简笔画面孔注意分配 Working Memory; Schematic Face; Attention Allocation
摘要: 本文通过采用记忆任务和视觉搜索任务相结合的方式,以简笔画情绪面孔为材料,探讨了视觉工作记忆内容对情绪面孔视觉搜索的影响。结果发现:不同工作记忆内容条件下对情绪面孔搜索的反应时存在显著差异,对两种情绪面孔搜索的反应时在无效信息条件下显著慢于有效信息条件下。结果表明,工作记忆内容能够引导情绪面孔的视觉搜索,实验结果拓展了偏向竞争理论模型的适用范围。
Abstract:  With the combined memory task and visual search paradigm, this study aimed to discuss the influence of contents of visual working memory to visual search of emotional faces. The results showed that the effect of the contents of visual working memory was significant. And the RTs were significantly longer in the invalid condition than in the valid condition for not only happy face but also angry face. The results suggested that the information which was maintained in visual working memory could affect the visual search efficiency of emotional faces. The findings broaden biased competition model.
文章引用:范玲霞, CodyDing, 郭仁露, 杨东 (2014). 视觉工作记忆内容引导情绪面孔的视觉搜索. 心理学进展, 4(1), 104-109. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2014.41018

参考文献

[1] 张豹, 金志成, 陈彩琦(2008). 视觉工作记忆对前注意阶段注意定向的调节. 心理学报, 5期, 552-561.
[2] Anderson, D. E., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2013). A common discrete resource for visual working memory and visual search. Psychological Science, 24, 929-938.
[3] Ashwin, C., Holas, P., Broadhurst, S., Kokoszka, A., Georgiou, G. A., & Fox, E. (2012). Enhanced anger superiority effect in generalized anxiety disorder and panic disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 329-336.
[4] Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). Automatic and strategic effects in the guidance of attention by working memory representations. Acta Psychologica, 137, 217-225.
[5] Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308-313.
[6] Chen, Z., & Tsou, B. H. (2011). Task-based working memory guidance of visual attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 10821095.
[7] Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2011). The contents of visual working memory reduce uncertainty during visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 996-1002.
[8] Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.
[9] Downing, P., & Dodds, C. (2004). Competition in visual working memory for control of search. Visual Cognition, 11, 689-703.
[10] Downing, P. E. (2000). Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychological Science, 11, 467-473.
[11] Eldar, S., Yankelevitch, R., Lamy, D., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Enhanced neural reactivity and selective attention to threat in anxiety. Biological Psychology, 85, 252-257.
[12] Gil-Gómez de Liaño, B., Botella, J., & Pascual-Ezama, D. (2011). The types of stimuli loaded in memory can modulate its effects on visual search. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 531-542.
[13] Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2009). The role of visual working memory (VWM) in the control of gaze during visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 936-949.
[14] Horstmann, G. (2007). Preattentive face processing: What do visual search experiments with schematic faces tell us? Visual Cognition, 15, 799-833.
[15] Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). The effect of items in working memory on the deployment of attention and the eyes during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 423-442.
[16] Kumar, S., Soto, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence for attentional guidance by the contents of working memory. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 307-317.
[17] Olivers, C. N., Meijer, F., & Theeuwes, J. (2006). Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: Visual working memory content affects visual attention. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1243-1265.
[18] Pan, Y., & Luo, Q. Y. (2012). Working memory modulates the perception of time. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 46-51.
[19] Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Working-memory capacity predicts the executive control of visual search among distractors: The influences of sustained and selective attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1430-1454.
[20] Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 248-261.
[21] Soto, D., Hodsoll, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic guidance of attention from working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 342-348.
[22] Soto, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Heinke, D. (2006). Working memory can guide pop-out search. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Vision Research, 46, 1010-1018.
[23] Soto, D., Wriglesworth, A., Bahrami-Balani, A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2010). Working memory enhances visual perception: Evidence from signal detection analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 441-456.
[24] Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 363-377.