威胁感知在外群体心理本质论与跨民族交往关系中的中介作用
The Mediation Effect of Threat Perception between Psychological Essentialism of Outgroup and Cross-Ethnic Interaction
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2014.44082, PDF, HTML, 下载: 2,904  浏览: 10,454  国家科技经费支持
作者: 杨晓莉, 梁筱媛:西北师范大学心理学院,西北师范大学西北少数民族教育发展研究中心,兰州;刘 力:北京师范大学心理学院,应用实验心理北京市重点实验室,北京
关键词: 威胁感知外群体心理本质论跨民族交往藏族Threat Perception Psychological Essentialism of Outgroup Cross-Ethnic Interaction Tibetan College Students
摘要: 为了探讨威胁感知在外群体的心理本质论与跨民族交往关系中的中介作用,该研究随机选取中国内地的113名藏族大学生采用问卷法进行了研究。结果表明,外群体的心理本质论越强,感知到的威胁越强,民族间的社会距离感也就越大,群际关系亲密度知觉相对较疏远;而且,威胁感知在外群体心理本质论与跨民族交往之间起了完全中介作用
Abstract: 113 Tibetan college students in Chinese inland were randomly recruited for this study to explore the mediation effect of threat perception between psychological essentialism of outgroup and cross-ethnic interaction using several questionnaires. The results showed that there were positive correlation between essentialism of outgroup and threat perception, and negative correlation between essentialism of outgroup and perceived social distance; threat perception was the whole mediator between psychological essentialism of outgroup and cross-ethnic interaction.
文章引用:杨晓莉, 梁筱媛, 刘力 (2014). 威胁感知在外群体心理本质论与跨民族交往关系中的中介作用. 心理学进展, 4(4), 609-616. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2014.44082

参考文献

[1] 方杰, 张敏强(2012). 中介效应的点估计和区间估计: 乘积分布法, 非参数Bootstrap和MCMC法. 心理学报, 10期, 1408-1420.
[2] 汪向东, 王希林, 马弘(1999). 心理卫生评定量表手册. 北京: 中国心理卫生杂志社.
[3] 杨晓莉(2012). 民族的心理本质论与跨民族交往的关系. 博士论文, 北京: 北京师范大学.
[4] 张婍, 冯江平, 王二平(2009). 群际威胁的分类及其对群体偏见的影响. 心理科学进展, 2期, 473-480.
[5] 周浩, 龙立荣(2004). 共同方法偏差的统计检验与控制方法. 心理科学进展, 6期, 942-950.
[6] Andrew, J. H., Brittany, A. W., & Heather, L. D. (2011). Psychological essentialism and its association with stigmatization. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 95-100.
[7] Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of inter-personal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596-612.
[8] Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 228-235.
[9] Bogardus, D. (1985). Bogardus’s Social Distance Scale. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, 25-36.
[10] Gaertner, L. & Schopler, J. (1998). Perceived ingroup entitativity and ingroup bias: An interconnection of self and others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 963-980.
[11] Haslam, N., Bastian, B., Bain, P., & Kashima, Y. (2006). Psychological essentialism, implicit theories, and intergroup relations.Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9, 63-76.
[12] Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 113-127.
[13] Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In S. Vosniadou, & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and Analo- gical Reasoning (pp. 179-195). New York: Cambridge University Press.
[14] No, S., Hong, Y. Y., Liao, H. Y., Lee, K., Wood, D., & Chao, M. M. (2008). Lay theory of race affects and moderates Asian Americans’ responses toward American culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 991-1004.
[15] Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2006). Essentializing differences between women and men. Psychological Science, 17, 129- 135.
[16] Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Psychological essentialism of human categories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 202-206.
[17] Renfro, C., Duran, A., Stephan, W. G., & Clason, D. L. (2006). The role of threat in attitudes toward affirmative action and its beneficiaries1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 41-74.
[18] Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., & Gaertner, S. L. (2006) Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 336-353.
[19] Sawires, J. N., & Peacock, M. J. (2000). Symbolic racism and voting behavior on Proposition 2091. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2092-2099.
[20] Smedley, A., & Smedley, B. D. (2005). Race as biology is fiction, racism as a social problem is real: Anthropological and historical perspectives on the social construction of race. American Psychologist, 60, 16-26.
[21] Stephan, W. G., & Renfro, C. L. (2002). The role of threat in intergroup relations. In D. Mackie & E. Smith (Eds.), From Prejudice to Intergroup Emotions: Differentiated Reactions to Social Groups. New York: Psychology Press, 5, 55-72.
[22] Stephan, W. G., Renfro, C. L., Esses, V. M., Stephan, C. W., & Martin, T. (2005). The effects of feeling threatened on attitudes toward immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 1-19.
[23] Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2000). An integrated theory of prejudice. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination: The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., 29, 559-576.
[24] Williams, M. J., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2008). Biological conceptions of race and the motivation to cross racial boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1033-1047.
[25] Zagefka, H., Pehrson, S, Mole, C. M., & Chan, E. (2010). The effect of essentialism in settings of historic intergroup atrocities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 718-732.
[26] Zarate, M. A., Garcia, B., Garza, A. A., & Hitlan, R. T. (2004). Cultural threat and perceived realistic group conflict as dual predictors of prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 99-105.