权力感对替代性内疚的影响
The Effect of Power on Vicarious Guilt
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2017.72021, PDF, HTML, XML, 下载: 1,706  浏览: 5,203  科研立项经费支持
作者: 陈佳芳, 张进辅:西南大学心理学部,重庆
关键词: 权力感替代性内疚Power Vicarious Guilt
摘要: 目的:探究权力感对替代性内疚的影响。方法:选取160名大学生为被试,采用情境法,并用到了个人权力感量表测量被试的主观权力感。结果:权力感对替代性内疚感的简单回归分析表明权力感的预测作用显著,β = 0.266,t = 3.473,P < 0.01。结论:个体的权力感越高,其在他人不道德行为上体验到的替代性内疚感越强。
Abstract: Objective: To explore the relationship between power and vicarious guilt. Methods: One hundred and sixty college students were recruited in the study, and they imagined a target doing an immoral behavior; they also completed emotional items to measure emotions and the personal sense of power scale to measure their subjective power. Results: Power could significantly predict vicarious guilt, β = 0.266, t = 3.473, P < 0.01. Conclusion: The higher level of power individuals have, the higher level of vicarious guilt they experience for others’ immoral behaviors.
文章引用:陈佳芳, 张进辅 (2017). 权力感对替代性内疚的影响. 心理学进展, 7(2), 171-175. https://doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.72021

参考文献

[1] 魏秋江, 段锦云, 范庭卫(2012). 权力操作范式的分析与比较. 心理科学进展, 20(9), 1507-1518.
[2] Anderson, C., John, O. P., & Keltner, D. (2012). The Personal Sense of Power. Journal of Personality, 80, 313-344. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00734.x
[3] De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral Sentiments and Cooperation: Differential Influences of Shame and Guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1025-1042. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600980874
[4] Ebenbach, D. H., & Keltner, D. (1998). Power, Emotion, and Judgmental Accuracy in Social Conflict: Motivating the Cognitive Miser. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 7-21. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp2001_2
[5] Eskine, K. J., Novreske, A., & Richards, M. (2013). Moral Contagion Effects in Everyday Interpersonal Encounters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 947-950. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.009
[6] Fast, N. J., Sivanathan, N., Mayer, N. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2012). Power and Overconfident Decision-Making. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 117, 249-260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.009
[7] Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social Beings: A Core Motives Approach to Social Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
[8] Haidt, J., & Rodin, J. (1999). Control and Efficacy as Interdisciplinary Bridges. Review of General Psychology, 3, 317-337. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.3.4.317
[9] Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, Approach, and Inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265- 284. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265
[10] Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The Effects of Feelings of Guilt on the Behaviour of Uncooperative Individuals in Repeated Social Bargaining Games: An Affect-as-Information Interpretation of the Role of Emotion in Social Interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 429-453. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000662
[11] Kipnis, D. (1972). Does Power Corrupt? Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 24, 33-41. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033390
[12] Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Scarnier, M., & Ames, D. R. (2005). Vicarious Shame and Guilt. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8, 145-157. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430205051064
[13] Magee, J. C., & Smith, P. K. (2013). The Social Distance Theory of Power. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 158-186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472732
[14] Scarnier, M., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2009). Parental Shame and Guilt: Distinguishing Emotional Responses to a Child’s Wrongdoings. Personal Relationships, 16, 205-220. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2009.01219.x
[15] Schmid Mast, M. (2010). Interpersonal Behaviour and Social Perception in a Hierarchy: The Interpersonal Power and Behaviour Model. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 1-33. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2010.486942
[16] Stets, J. E., & Turner, J. H. (2006). Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions. New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-30715-2
[17] Tangney, J. P., & Fischer, K. W. (1997). Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 185, 55-59.
[18] Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral Emotions and Moral Behavior. Social Science Electronic Publishing, 58, 345-372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
[19] Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed into Anger? The Relation of Shame and Guilt to Anger and Self-Reported Aggression. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 62, 669-675. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.669
[20] Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Stereotypes about Sentiments and Status: Emotional Expectations for High- and Low-Status Group Members. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 560-574. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200267004
[21] Wicker, F. W., Payne, G. C., & Morgan, R. D. (1983). Participant Descriptions of Guilt and Shame. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 25-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00992963
[22] Winter, D. G. (1973). The Power Motive. New York: Free Press.