英语新词学习认知机制的研究进展——语音工作记忆还是语音敏感性?
The Research Advance for the Cognitive Mechanism of English New Word Learning—Phonological Loop or Phonological Sensitivity?
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2014.41012, PDF, HTML, 下载: 3,374  浏览: 15,892  国家自然科学基金支持
作者: 程晓荣, 范 炤, 周宗奎:青少年网络心理与行为教育部重点实验室,武汉;华中师范大学心理学院,武汉
关键词: 新词学习非词重复语音工作记忆语音敏感性词汇重构New Word Learning; Nonword Repetition; Phonological Working Memory; Phonological Sensitivity; Lexical Restructuring
摘要:  新词学习的两个主要理论为语音回路模型和词汇重构理论,它们分别侧重于对新词学习影响的一个方面:前者侧重于语音工作记忆,后者侧重于语音加工中的语音敏感性。研究表明语音工作记忆和语音敏感性都对新词学习有重要影响,特别是语音工作记忆直接影响了新词学习,但是语音敏感性对新词学习的影响机制仍不清楚。进一步的研究可以采用新的研究方法来研究语音工作记忆和语音敏感性对新词学习的作用机制或者把两者统和起来研究新词学习
Abstract:  Two main theories in new word learning were Phonological Loop Model and Lexical Restructuring Theory. The two main theories focused on different aspects of how long-term lexical knowledge affects word learning: Phonological Loop Model focusing on phonological working memory and Lexical Restructuring Theory focusing on phonological sensitivity in phonological processing. Previous studies showed that both phonological working memory and phonological sensitivity were important to new word learning, especially phonological working memory influencing word learning directly. However, how phonological sensitivity affects word learning is still not clear. Future studies could use a new method to investigate the mechanism of how phonological working memory and phonological sensitivity affect word learning or integrate the two aspects to investigate word learning.
文章引用:程晓荣, 范炤, 周宗奎 (2014). 英语新词学习认知机制的研究进展——语音工作记忆还是语音敏感性?. 心理学进展, 4(1), 64-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2014.41012

参考文献

[1] Abbeduto, L., Warren, S. F., & Connors, F. A. (2007). Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13, 247-261.
[2] Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice (2 Edition). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
[3] Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158-173.
[4] Baddeley, A. D., Pa-pagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 576-596.
[5] Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 44-78.
[6] Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441-469.
[7] Brown, G. D. A., & Hulme, C. (1996). Nonword repetition, STM, and word age-of-acquisition: A computational model. In: S. E. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of Short-Term Memory (pp. 129-148). Hove: Psychology Press.
[8] Crano, W. D., & Mellon, P. M. (1978). Causal influence of teachers’ expectations of children’s academic performance: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 39-49.
[9] Dollaghan, C. A., Biber, M. E., & Campbell, T. F. (1993). Constituent syllables effects in a nonsense-word repetition task. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1051-1054.
[10] Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sound in early language acquisition: English initial consonants in the first fifty words. Language, 51, 419-439.
[11] Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 83-94.
[12] Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513-543.
[13] Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1994). Children’s phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672-688.
[14] Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: Is there a causal learning? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.
[15] Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439-454.
[16] Gathercole, S. E., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., Tiffany, C., & Team, A. S. (2008). Deficits in verbal long-term memory and learning in children with poor phonological short-term memory skills. The Quarterly Journal of Experi-mental Psychology, 61, 474-490.
[17] Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966-979.
[18] Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school year: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887-898.
[19] Gorman, B. K. (2012). Relationships between vocabulary size, working memory, and phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21, 109.
[20] Gupta, P., & Tisdale, J. (2009). Does phonological short-term memory causally determine vocabulary learning? Toward a computational resolution of the debate. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 481502.
[21] Gupta, P., & Tisdale, J. (2009). Word learning, phonological short-term memory, phonotactic probability and long-term memory: Towards an integrated framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3755-3771.
[22] Jarrold, C., Thorn, A. S. C., & Stephens, E. (2009). The relationships among verbal short-term memory, phonological awareness, and new word learning: Evidence from typical development and Down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 196-218.
[23] Jones, G., & Witherstone, H. L. (2011). Lexical and sublexical knowledge influences the encoding, storage, and articulation of nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 399, 588-599.
[24] Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’s sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Child Language, 33, 630-645.
[25] Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Phonological short-term memory and foreign vocabulary learning International Journal of psychology, 34, 383-388.
[26] Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306-323.
[27] Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 3-19.
[28] Metsala, J. L., & Chisholm, G. M. (2010). The influence of lexical status and neighborhood density on children’s nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 489-506.
[29] Metsala, J. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). An examination of young children’s phonological processing as a function of lexical development. The Annual American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
[30] Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representation: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In: J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word Recognition in Beginning Literacy (pp. 89-120). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
[31] Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 78-94.
[32] Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (2009). A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3737-3753.
[33] Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1992). Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 44A, 47-67.
[34] Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2011). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113-125.
[35] Pisoni, D. B., & Luce, P. A. (1987). Acoustic-phonetic representations in word recognition. Cognition, 25, 21-52.
[36] Ramachandra, V., Hewitt, L. E., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). The relationship between phonological memory, phonological sensitivity, and incidental word learning. Journal of Psycholinguitic Research, 40, 93-109.
[37] Roch, M., & Jarrold, C. (2008). A comparison between word and nonword reading in Down syndrome: The role of phonological awareness. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 305-318.
[38] Tamburelli, M., Jones, G., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M. (2012). Computational modelling of phonological acquisition: Simulating error patterns in nonword repetition tasks. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 901-946.
[39] Thorn, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Frankish, C. (2005). Redintegration and the benefits of long-term knowledge in verbal short-term memory: An evaluation of Schweickert’s (1993) multinomial processing tree model. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 133-158.