姓名字母效应:内隐自尊的一种反映指标
Name Letter Effect—An Index of Implicit Self-Esteem
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2014.42041, PDF, HTML, 下载: 3,162  浏览: 14,267  科研立项经费支持
作者: 顾念君, 杨红升, 王 芳:西南大学心理学部,重庆
关键词: 姓名字母效应内隐自尊外显自尊内隐自我中心Name-Letter Effect; Implicit Esteem; Explicit Esteem; Implicit Egotism
摘要: 姓名字母效应表现为个人对于自己名字的组成字符有着相对于其他字符的偏好,反映了个体对于自我以及和自我有关的事物的内隐积极态度,常被作为考察内隐自尊的一个重要指标。文章介绍了姓名字母效应的研究程序与记分方法、以姓名字母偏好任务研究内隐自尊的有关发现以及姓名字母效应对于实际生活决策和任务成绩的影响,并在展望部分提出了需要进一步研究的问题及在中文语境下研究姓名字母效应的可行性。
Abstract: Name-letter effect refers to the phenomenon that people evaluate the letters in their own names more favorably than letters that are not in their own names. It reflects an implicit positive attitude towards the self and self-related objects, thus, it can be used as an index of implicit self-esteem. The current paper introduced the procedures and scoring methods for name-letter effect, findings of using name-letter task, as well as its implications in life decisions and task performances. Two issues that remain for future research and the feasibility of studying name-letter effect in Chinese context were discussed.
文章引用:顾念君, 杨红升, 王芳. 姓名字母效应:内隐自尊的一种反映指标[J]. 心理学进展, 2014, 4(2): 284-293. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2014.42041

参考文献

[1] 陈敏燕, 史慧颖, 陈红(2009). 贫困大学生内隐自尊状况分析. 中国学校卫生, 30(3), 229-230.
[2] Anseel, F., & Duyck, W. (2008). Unconscious applicants: A systematic test of the name-letter effect. Psychological Science, 19, 1059-1061.
[3] Arnell, K.M., Shapiro, K.L., & Sorensen, R.E. (1999). Reduced repetition blindness for one's own name. Visual Cognition, 6, 609-635.
[4] Baccus, J.R., Baldwin, M.W., & Packer, D.J. (2004). Increasing implicit self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15, 498-502.
[5] Bosson, J.K., Swann Jr., W.B., & Pennebaker, J.W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631-645.
[6] Brendl, C.M., Chattopadhyay, A., Pelham, B.W., & Carvallo, M. (2005). Name letter branding: Valence transfers when product specific needs are active. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 405-415.
[7] Christenfeld, N., Phillips, D.P., & Glynn, L.M. (1999). What’s in a name: Mortality and the power of symbols. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 241-254.
[8] DeHart, T. (2002). The hidden effects of early experiences: The origins and stability of implicit self-esteem. Unpublished doctoral Dissertation, The State University of New York at Buffalo.
[9] De Raedt, R., Schacht, R., Franck, E., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Self-esteem and depression revisited: Implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1017-1028.
[10] Fischer, C., Dailler, F., & Morlet, D. (2008). Novelty P3 elicited by the subject’s own name in comatose patients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 2224-2230.
[11] Gallucci, M. (2003). I sell seashells by the seashore and my name is Jack: Comment on Pelham, Mirenberg, and Jones (2002). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 789-799.
[12] Gawronski, B., Bodenhausen, G.V., & Becker, A.P. (2007). I like it, because I like myself: Associative self-anchoring and post-decisional change of implicit evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 221-232.
[13] Gebauer, J.E., Leary, M.R., & Neberich, W. (2012). Unfortunate first names effects of name-based relational devaluation and interpersonal neglect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 590-596.
[14] Greenwald, A.G., & Banaji, M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27.
[15] Hall, G.S. (1898). Some aspects of the early sense of self. The American Journal of Psychology, 9, 351-395.
[16] Harari, H., & McDavid, J.W. (1973). Name stereotypes and teachers’ expectations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 222-225.
[17] Hoorens, V., & Nuttin, J.M. (1993). Overvaluation of own attributes: Mere ownership or subjective frequency? Social Cognition, 11, 177-200.
[18] Hoorens, V., Nuttin, J.M., Herman, I.E., & Pavakanun, U. (1990). Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership: A quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross-alphabetical test of the name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 181-205.
[19] Jones, J.T., Pelham, B.W., Carvallo, M., & Mirenberg, M.C. (2004). How do I love thee? Let me count the Js: Implicit egotism and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 665-683.
[20] Jones, J.T., Pelham, B.W., Mirenberg, M.C., & Hetts, J.J. (2002). Name letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism as self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 170-177.
[21] Kitayama, S., & Rarasawa, M. (1997). Implicit self-esteem in Japan: Name letters and birthday numbers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 736-742.
[22] Knewtson, H.S., & Sias, R.W. (2010). Why Susie owns Starbucks: The name letter effect in security selection. Journal of Business Research, 63, 1324-1327.
[23] Koole, S.L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 669-685.
[24] Koole, S.L., & Pelham, B.W. (2003). On the nature of implicit self-esteem: The case of the name letter effect. In Motivated social perception: The Ontario symposium, 9(93-116). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah.
[25] Krause, S., Back, M.D., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S.C. (2011). Reliability of implicit self-esteem measures revisited. European Journal of Personality, 25, 239-251.
[26] Krizan, Z. (2008). What is implicit about implicit self-esteem? Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1635-1640.
[27] Krizan, Z., & Suls, J. (2008). Are implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem related? A meta-analysis for the Name-Letter Test. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 521-531.
[28] LeBel, E.P., & Gawronski, B. (2009). How to find what’s in a name: Scrutinizing the optimality of five scoring algorithms for the name-letter task. European Journal of Personality, 23, 85-106.
[29] McCullough, B.D., & McWilliams, T.P. (2010). Baseball players with the initial “K” do not strike out more often. Journal of Applied Statistics, 37, 881-891.
[30] McCullough, B.D., & McWilliams, T.P. (2011). Students with the initial “A” don’t get better grades. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 340-343.
[31] Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56-60.
[32] Nelson, L.D., & Simmons, J.P. (2007). Moniker maladies when names sabotage success. Psychological Science, 18, 11061112.
[33] Nuttin, J.M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 353-361.
[34] Nuttin, J.M. (1987). Affective consequences of mere ownership: The name letter effect in twelve European languages. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 381-402.
[35] Pelham, B.W., Koole, S.L., Hardin, C.D., Hetts, J.J., Seah, E., & DeHart, T. (2005). Gender moderates the relation between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 84-89.
[36] Pelham, B.W., Mirenberg, M.C., & Jones, J.T. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 469-487.
[37] Rudolph, A., Schröder-Abé, M., Schütz, A., Gregg, A.P., & Sedikides, C. (2008). Through a glass, less darkly? Reassessing convergent and divergent validity in measures of implicit self-esteem. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 273-281.
[38] Sakellaropoulo, M., & Baldwin, M.W. (2007). The hidden sides of self-esteem: Two dimensions of implicit self-esteem and their relation to narcissistic reactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 995-1001.
[39] Schröder-Abé, M., Rudolph, A., Wiesner, A., & Schütz, A. (2007). Self-esteem discrepancies and defensive reactions to social feedback. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 174-183.
[40] Shapiro, K.L., Caldwell, J., & Sorensen, R.E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 504-514.
[41] Shimizu, M., & Pelham, B.W. (2011). Liking for positive words and icons moderates the association between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 994-999.
[42] Simonsohn, U. (2011). Spurious? Name similarity effects (implicit egotism) in marriage, job, and moving decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1-61.
[43] Stieger, S., & Burger, C. (2013). More complex than previously thought: New insights into the optimal administration of the initial preference task. Self and Identity, 12, 201-216.
[44] Stieger, S., & LeBel, E.P. (2012). Name-letter preferences for new last name and abandoned birth name initials in the context of name-change via marriage. Social Psychology, 43, 7-13.
[45] Stieger, S., Voracek, M., & Formann, A.K. (2012). How to administer the initial preference task. European Journal of Personality, 26, 63-78.
[46] Verplanken, B., Friborg, O., Wang, C., Trafimow, D., & Woolf, K. (2007). Mental habits: Metacognitive reflection on negative self-thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 526-541.
[47] Wentura, D., Kulfanek, M., & Greve, W. (2005). Masked affective priming by name letters: Evidence for a correspondence of explicit and implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 654-663.
[48] Wilson, T.D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T.Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101-126.
[49] Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151-175.