预测水平、刺激性质在条件性恐惧形成中的分离与交互效应
Dissociation and Interaction between Prediction Levels and Stimulus Properties in Conditioned Fear
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2014.43056, PDF, HTML, 下载: 2,753  浏览: 6,221  科研立项经费支持
作者: 曾祥星, 郑楚华, 庄楚群, 杜 娟, 郑希付:华南师范大学心理学院,广州
关键词: 预测水平刺激性质条件性恐惧分离效应Prediction Level Stimulus Properties Conditioned Fear Dissociation Effect
摘要: 不仅刺激性质引起条件性恐惧,而且预测水平也能引起条件性恐惧,那么二者之间存什么样的分离与交互作用呢?对此,采用事件相关电位,把预测水平分为可预测与不可预测两个水平,刺激性质分为中性刺激和负性刺激。采用2 × 2被试内实验设计,探索预测水平、刺激性质在条件性恐惧形成中的分离效应。实验结果发现:在可预测水平下,刺激性质之间ERP波形在加工早期就存在显著差异,出现了分离;在不可预测水平下,刺激性质之间的ERP波形在加工早期差异不显著;不同刺激性质在不同预测水平上ERP波形差异显著。这些结果表明预测水平、刺激性质在条件性恐惧形成中存在分离与交互效应。
Abstract: Conditioned fear is caused by not only stimulus properties but also prediction level. Then what dissociation and interaction are between prediction levels and stimulus properties? Using event-related potentials (ERP), prediction levels were divided as unprediction and prediction; stimulus properties were divided as neutral and aversion stimulus. Use 2 × 2 within-subjects experimental design, to explore the dissociation between prediction levels and stimulus properties in the acquisition phase of conditioned fear. These data found that: the ERP between the stimulus properties had a significant difference at an early stage of processing under prediction; the ERP between the stimulus properties did not have a significant difference at an early stage of processing under unprediction; the ERP of different stimulus properties had a significant difference under different prediction levels. The results showed there were dissociation and interaction between prediction levels and stimulus properties in conditioned fear.
文章引用:曾祥星, 郑楚华, 庄楚群, 杜娟, 郑希付 (2014). 预测水平、刺激性质在条件性恐惧形成中的分离与交互效应. 心理学进展, 4(3), 400-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2014.43056

参考文献

[1] 庄锦英(2006). 决策心理学. 上海: 上海教育出版社.
[2] Badia, P., Suter, S., & Lewis, P. (1967). Preference for warned shock: Information and/or preparation. Psychological Reports, 20, 271-274.
[3] Bouton, M. E. (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 976 -986.
[4] Bouton, M. E., García-Gutiérrez, A., Zilski, J., & Moody, E. W. (2006). Extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily make extinction less vulnerable to relapse. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 983-994.
[5] Cantor, M. B., & LoLordo, V. M. (1970). Rats prefer signaled reinforcing brain stimulation to unsignaled ESB. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 183-191.
[6] Davis, M., Walker, D. L., & Myers, K. M. (2003). Role of the amygdala in fear extinction measured with potentiated startle. Annals of the New York Academy Sciences, 985, 218-232.
[7] Foa, E. B., Zinbarg, R., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1992). Uncontrollability and unpredictability in post-traumatic stress disorder. An animal model. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 218-238.
[8] Felmingham, K. L., Williams, L. M., Kemp, A. H., Rennie, C., Gordon, E., & Bryant, R. A. (2009). Anterior cingulate activity to salient stimuli is modulated by autonomic arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 173, 59-62.
[9] Grillon, C., Baas, J. P., Lissek, S., Smith, K., & Milstein, J. (2004). Anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable aversive events. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 916-924.
[10] Grillon, C., Lissek, S., Rabin, S., McDowell, D., Dvir, S., & Pine, D. S. (2008). Increased anxiety during anticipation of unpredictable but not predictable aversive stimuli as a psychophysiologic marker of panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 898-904.
[11] Grillon, C., Daniel, S., Lissek, P. S., Rabin, S., Bonne, O., & Vythilingam, M. (2009). Increased anxiety during anticipation of unpredictable aversive stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder but not in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 47-53.
[12] Jin, Y., Yan, K., Zheng, Y. H., Jiang, Y. J., Tao, R., & Zheng, X. F. (2013). Gender differences in detecting unanticipated stimuli: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 538, 38-42.
[13] Kim, J. J., & Jung, M. W. (2006). Neural circuits and mechanisms involved in Pavlovian fear conditioning: A critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 188-202.
[14] Kindt, M., & Soeter, M. (2013). Reconsolidation in a human fear conditioning study: A test of extinction as updating mechanism. Biological Psychology, 91, 43-50.
[15] Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report, A-8.
[16] LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155-184.
[17] Lockard, J. S. (1965). Choice of a warning signal or none in several unavoidable shock situations. Psychonomic Science, 3, 5-6.
[18] Maren, S. (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 897-931.
[19] Moberg, C. A., & Crutin, J. J. (2009). Alcohol selectively reduces anxiety but not fear: Startle response during unpredictable vs. predictable threat. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 335-347.
[20] Pervin, L. A. (1963). The need to predict and control under conditions of threat. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 570-585.
[21] Rescorla, R. A. (2004). Spontaneous recovery. Learning & Memory, 11, 501-509.
[22] Seligman, M. E. P., & Binik, Y. M. (1977). The safety-signal hypothesis. In H. Davis & H. M. B. Hurwitz (Eds.), Operant-Pavlovian Interactions (pp. 165-187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[23] Suna, N., Qua, C., Zhaoa, S. C., Yua, L. Y., & Zheng, X. F. (2011). Allocation of attention in response to novel neutral stimuli and predictive negative stimuli in men and women: An event-related potentials research study. Biological Rhythm Research, 1, 1-9.
[24] Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lusthene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
[25] Weiss, J. M. (1971). Effects of coping behavior in different warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 77, 1-13.