反应风格与人格特质的关系
The Relationship between Response Styles and Personality Traits
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2016.63042, PDF, HTML, XML, 下载: 2,147  浏览: 9,167 
作者: 李 盟, 郭庆科:山东师范大学心理学院,山东 济南
关键词: 反应风格大五人格受教育程度高中生大学生Response Style Big Five Personality The Level of Education High School Students Undergraduates
摘要: 反应风格是指被试基于某些固定的倾向而不是基于具体的测验内容对题目做出选择。传统观点认为反应风格是一种系统的误差,而新近的观点发现反应风格很有可能是一种独特的人格变量。鉴于此,本研究在后者的视角下试图检验反应风格的类特质属性以及反应风格的影响因素。本研究采用大五人格问卷NEO-FFI以及语义反转的NEO-FFI分别对1200名高中生和1200名大学生进行了前后两次施测,时间间隔为1个月。计算两次测验中同一对题目相同答案的次数为反应风格的指标。结果发现:1) 中国被试默认肯定反应风格最多,依次是默认否定、折中、极端否定和极端肯定风格,且各种反应风格并不是独立存在的。2) 随着受教育程度的提高,各种反应风格均显著下降。性别在默认肯定风格和折中风格上主效应显著,性别与受教育程度在极端肯定风格以及折中风格上的交互作用显著。3) 各反应风格与人格特质的不同维度均存在不同程度和不同方向的相关。
Abstract: Response styles refer to respondents’ systematic tendency to answer questionnaire items on some basis other than the specific item content. The traditional opinions view response styles as a kind of system error, but the new viewpoints find that the response styles are more likely to be unique personality variables. Due to this, the present study is trying to examine the trait-like properties and the effect factors of response styles in the latter perspectives. This research adopts the Big Five Personality questionnaire NEO-FFI and semantic reversal of NEO-FFI measures 1200 high school students and 1200 undergraduates twice one month in between calculating the number of the same answer at each pair of items as response styles indicators in two tests. The findings were as follows: 1) The acquiescent response style is more than other response styles in Chinese respondents, then the disacquiescence response style, midpoint response style, positive extreme response style and negative extreme response style. All kinds of response styles are not independent. 2) All response styles were significantly reduced with the higher education. The main effect of gender on the acquiescent response style and midpoint response style were significant, and the interaction effect was significant on midpoint response style and positive extreme response style. 3) Each response styles were related to personality traits at different degree and direction.
文章引用:李盟, 郭庆科 (2016). 反应风格与人格特质的关系. 心理学进展, 6(3), 320-331. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2016.63042

参考文献

[1] 郭庆科, 王昭, 韩丹, 时勘(2007). 中国高中学生中的反应风格及其效应. 心理学报, 39(1), 176-183.
[2] 余益兵, 邹泓, 曲可佳(2008). 中学生反应风格的特点及其在人格与自尊关系中的效应. 中国临床心理学杂志, 16, 544-546.
[3] 郑湘萍(2003). 论儒家中庸观及其现代意义. 桂海论丛, 19(2), 62-65.
[4] Austin, E. J., Deary, I. J., & Egan, V. (2006). Individual Differences in Response Scale Use: Mixed Rasch Modelling of Responses to NEO-FFI Items. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1235-1245.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.018
[5] Bachman, J. G., & O’Malley, P. M. (1984). Yea-Saying, Nay-Saying, and Going to Extremes: Black-White Differences in Response Styles. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 491-509.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/268845
[6] Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised Neo Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Psychological Assessment Resources.
[7] Couch, A., & Ke-niston, K. (1960). Yea-Sayers and Nay-Sayers: Agreeing Response Set as a Personality Variable. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 151-172.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040372
[8] Cronbach, L. J. (1946). Response Sets and Test Validity. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6, 475-494.
[9] Cronbach, L. J. (1950). Further Evidence on Response Sets and Test Design. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 10, 3-31.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316445001000101
[10] De Jong, M. G., Steenkamp, J. B., Fox, J. P., & Baumgartner, H. (2008). Using Item Response Theory to Measure Extreme Response Style in Marketing Research: A Global Investigation. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 104-115.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.45.1.104
[11] Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to Account for Cross-Cultural Re-sponse Bias: A Classification of Score Adjustment Procedures and Review of Research in JCCP. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 263-282.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022104264122
[12] Gibbons, J. L., Zellner, J. A. and Rudek, D. J. (1999) Effects of Language and Meaningfulness on the Use of Extreme Response Style by Spanish-English Bilinguals. Cross-Cultural Re-search, 33, 369-381.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/106939719903300404
[13] Graziano, W. G., & Tobin, R. M. (2002). Agreeableness: Di-mension of Personality or Social Desirability Artifact? Journal of Personality, 70, 695-728.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.05021
[14] Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex Differences in Intelligence. Implications for Education. American Psychologist, 52, 1091-1102.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1091
[15] Harzing, A.-W. (2006). Response Styles in Cross-National Survey Research: A 26-Country Study. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 6, 243-266.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1470595806066332
[16] He, J., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2013). A General Response Style Factor: Evidence from a Multi-Ethnic Study in the Netherlands. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 794-800.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.017
[17] He, J., Bartram, D., Inceoglu, I., & van de Vijver, F. J. (2014a). Response Styles and Personality Traits: A Multilevel Analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 1028-1045.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022114534773
[18] He, J., Vijver, F. J. R. V. D., Espinosa, A. D., & Mui, P. H. C. (2014b). Toward a Unification of Acquiescent, Extreme, and Midpoint Response Styles a Multilevel Study. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1470595814541424
[19] Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex Differences in Mental Test Scores, Variability, and Numbers of High-Scoring Individuals. Science, 269, 41-45.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7604277
[20] Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of Culture and Response Format on Extreme Response Style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 296-309.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022189203004
[21] Kim, M. S. (1994). Cross-Cultural Comparisons of the Perceived Importance of Interactive Constraints. Human Communication Research, 21, 128-151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1994.tb00343.x
[22] Kim, M. S., Hunter, J. E., Miyahara, A., Horvath, A. M., Bresnahan, M., & Yoon, H. J. (1996). Individual versus Culture-Level Dimensions of Individualism-Collectivism: Effects on Preferred Conversational Styles. Communication Monographs, 63, 29-49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637759609376373
[23] Lentz, T. F. (1938). Acquiescence as a Factor in the Measurement of Personality. Psychological Bulletin, 35, 659.
[24] Light, C. S., Zax, M., & Gardiner, D. H. (1965). Relationship of Age, Sex, and Intelligence Level to Extreme Response Style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 907-909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0022746
[25] Marin, G., Gamba, R. J., & Marin, B. V. (1992). Extreme Response Style and Acquiescence among Hispanics. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23, 498-509.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022192234006
[26] Meisenberg, G., & Williams, A. (2008). Are Acquiescent and Extreme Response Styles Related to Low Intelligence and Education? Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1539-1550.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.01.010
[27] Mick, D. G. (1996). Are Studies of Dark Side Variables Confounded by Socially Desirable Responding? The Case of Materialism. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 106-119.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209470
[28] Moors, G. (2008). Exploring the Effect of a Middle Response Category on Response Style in Attitude Measurement. Quality & Quantity, 42, 779-794.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11135-006-9067-x
[29] Moors, G. (2010). Ranking the Ratings: A Latent-Class Regression Model to Control for Overall Agreement in Opinion Research. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 22, 93-119.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edp036
[30] Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and Control of Response Biases. In J. Robinson, P. Shaver, & L. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes (Vol. 1, pp. 17-59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-590241-0.50006-X
[31] Smith, P. B. (2004). Acquiescent Response Bias as an Aspect of Cultural Communication Style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 50-61.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022103260380
[32] Smith, P. B. (2011). Communication Styles as Dimensions of Na-tional Culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 216-233.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110396866
[33] Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The Psychology of Survey Response. New York: Cambridge University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511819322
[34] Van Herk, H., Poortinga, Y. H., & Verhallen, T. M. (2004). Response Styles in Rating Scales Evidence of Method Bias in Data from Six EU Countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 346-360.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022104264126
[35] Van Vaerenbergh, Y., & Thomas, T. D. (2013). Response Styles in Survey Research: A Literature Review of Antecedents, Consequences, and Remedies. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 25, 195-217.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/eds021
[36] Weijters, B. (2006). Response Styles in Consumer Research. Doctoral Dissertation, Ghent: Faculty of Economics and Business Administration, Ghent University.
[37] Weijters, B., Geuens, M., & Schillewaert, N. (2010). The Stability of Individual Response Styles. Psychological Methods, 15, 96-110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018721