强迫症的认知表达与归纳推理的领域特殊性
Cognitive Expression of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Domain-Specificity of Induction
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2017.71007, PDF, HTML, XML, 下载: 1,689  浏览: 4,277  国家自然科学基金支持
作者: 蒋 柯:西南民族大学社会学与心理学学院,四川 成都 ;梁佩鹏:首都医科大学宣武医院,北京
关键词: 强迫症归纳推理领域特殊性信号检测理论治疗Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) Inductive Reasoning Domain-Specificity Theory of Signal-Detection Treatment
摘要: 文章综述了有关强迫症(Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, OCD)的病理生理学和认知心理学的研究进展,提出从领域特殊性的角度来理解强迫症的病理机制。通过引证归纳的领域特殊性研究,分析了强迫症认知偏差的领域特殊性特征,并提出假设:强迫症患者的认知偏差可能有两种类型,第一类认知偏差是对任务效价评估的程度误差;第二类认知偏差是对任务类别判断的错误。本研究聚焦于第二类认知偏差的论证。本文最后提出了关于强迫症的第二类认知偏差的实验研究假设:与健康对照相比,1) 强迫症患者在“获利”情景中的信号检查指标:报告标准(C)趋于宽松,分辨率(d’)较低;2) 强迫症患者在“获利”和“避害”两种任务情景中的信号检查指标没有显著差异。
Abstract: This article reviewed the research progress of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) in pathophysiology and cognitive psychology, analyzed the theoretical and practical difficulty from domain- general explanation of OCD, and then suggested approaching the pathophysiological mechanism of OCD from the domain-specificity aspect. By quoting the studies about domain specificity of induction, the domain-specificity characteristics of OCD were discussed and new hypotheses were proposed: there are two types of cognitive bias in OCD: the first is the wrong assessment of the task valence, and the second is the improper classification of task type. The first cognitive bias was widely explored, while the second one was never studied. In the end, this article put forward the experimental hypothesis about the second cognitive bias as follows. In contrast to healthy controls, 1) OCD patients may get looser report-criterion (C) and lower distinguishability (d’) under the condition of Embrace-Advantage; while 2) there may be no significant difference of signal-detec- tion indexes under the conditions of Embrace-Advantage and Resist-Disadvantage for OCD patients.
文章引用:蒋柯, 梁佩鹏 (2017). 强迫症的认知表达与归纳推理的领域特殊性. 心理学进展, 7(1), 42-49. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.71007

参考文献

[1] 戴维•巴斯(2015). 进化心理学. 张勇, 等, 译. 北京: 商务印书馆.
[2] 姜英杰, 王溢涵(2009). 强迫症的元认知研究: 进展与趋势. 心理科学, 32(4), 903-904.
[3] 蒋柯, 熊哲宏(2010a). 归纳推理是自然选择塑造的领域特殊性适应机制. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 28(3), 54- 60.
[4] 蒋柯, 熊哲宏(2010b). 利/害条件下归纳推理的心理效应分离. 心理学报, 42(11), 1050-1059.
[5] 蒋柯(2015). 归纳推理的心理学研究. 广州: 世界图书出版公司.
[6] 刘泱慧, 位照国, 刘铁榜(2014). 无法忍受不确定性的起源、发展及研究. 中国精神卫生杂志, 28(10), 796-801.
[7] 史继红, 张敬悬(2015). 抗抑郁药物联合星状神经节阻滞治疗躯体形式障碍临床评价. 精神医学杂志, 28(5), 354-358.
[8] 孙伯民, 李殿友, 朗黎琴, 潘力, 孙成彦, 李永超, 张海音, 赵永波, 沈建康(2003). 内囊前肢毁损术治疗难治性强迫症. 中国神经精神疾病杂志, 29(2), 81-85.
[9] 占世坤, 孙伯民(2012). 外源性磷酸肌酸对难治性强迫症术后认知功能的疗效观察. 工作探讨, 9(30), 159-162.
[10] 张众良, 张仲明, 李红(2010). 强迫症病理的认知—行为研究述评. 心理科学进展, 18(2), 306-313.
[11] 周强, 蒋柯(2009). 利/害条件下归纳推理的心理逻辑分离. 宁波教育学院学报, 11(2), 64-69.
[12] Abramovitch, A., & Coopermen, A. (2015). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Critical Review. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 5, 24-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.01.002
[13] American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition: DSM-5. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
[14] Bloch, M. H., Landeros-Wersenberger, A., Rosario, M. C., Pittenger, C., & Leckman, J. F. (2008). Meta-Analysis of the Symptom Structure of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1532-1542. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08020320
[15] Del Casale, A., Rapinesi, C., Brugnoli, R. et al. (2015). Executive Functions in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: An Activation Likelihood Estimate Meta-Analysis of fMRI Studies. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 8, 1-16.
[16] Dimaggia, G., Carcione, A., Salvatore, G., Nicolò, G., Sisto, A., & Semerari, A. (2011). Progressively Promoting Meta-Cognition in a Case of Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder Treated with Meta-Cognitive Interpersonal Therapy. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 84, 70-83.
[17] Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Aardema, A. (1999). Metacognition, Specific Obsessive-Compulsive Beliefs and Obsessive-Compulsive Behaviour. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 139-145. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199905)6:2<139::AID-CPP194>3.0.CO;2-9
[18] Eng, G. K., Sim, K., & Chen, S. A. (2015). Meta-Analytic Investigation of Structural Grey Matter, Executive Domain-Related Functional Activations, and White Mater Diffusivity in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: An Integrative Review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 52, 233-257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.002
[19] Gwilliam, P., Wells, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). Does Meta-Cognition or Responsibility Predict Obsessive-Com- pulsive Symptoms: A Test of the Meta-Cognitive Model. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11, 137-144. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.402
[20] Hezal, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (2015). A Theoretical Review of Cognitive Biases and Deficits in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychology, 121, 221-232.
[21] Jonsson, H., Kristensen, M., & Arendt, M. (2015). Intensive Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorder, 6, 83-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.04.004
[22] Jung, H. H., Kim, C. H., Chang, J. H., Park, Y. G., Chung, S. S., & Chang, J. W. (2006). Bilateral Anterior Cingulotomy for Refractory Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Long-Term Fol-low-Up Results. Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery, 84, 184-189. https://doi.org/10.1159/000095031
[23] Liew, J., & Hayes, B. K. (2013). Inductive and Deductive Reasoning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2890-2895). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
[24] McGuire, J. F., Piacentini, J., Lewin, A. B., Brennan, E. A., Murphy, T. K., & Storch, E. A. (2015). A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Medication for Child Compulsive Disorder: Moderators of Treatment Efficacy, Response, and Remission. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 580-593. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22389
[25] Modell, J. C., Moutz, J. M., Curtis, G. C., & Greden, J. F. (1989). Neuriophysiolgic Dysfunction in Basal Ganglia/Limbic Striatal and Thalamoccortical Circuits as a Pathogenetic Mechanism of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 1, 27-36.
[26] Moritz, S., Peters, M. J. V., Laror, F., & Lincoln, T. M. (2010). Meta-Cognitive Beliefs in Obsessive-Compulsive Patients: A Comparison with Healthy and Schizophrenia Participants. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 531-548. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546801003783508
[27] O’Connor, K. P., & Robillard, S. (1995). Inference Processes in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Some Clinical Observation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 887-896. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(95)00042-V
[28] Olatunji, B. O., Davis, M. L., Powers, M. B., & Smits, J. A. J. (2013). Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Obsessive-Com- pulsive Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Treatment Outcome and Moderators. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 33-41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.08.020
[29] Rachman, S. (2007). Self-Constructs in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 21, 257-262.
[30] Rosa-Alcázar, Á. I., Sánchez-Meca, J, Rosa-Alcázar, A, Iniesta-Sepúlveda, M., Olivares-Rodríguez, J., & Parada-Navas, J. L. (2015). Psychological Treatment of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in Children and Adolescents: A Meta-Analysis. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 18, 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2015.22
[31] Simpson, J., Cove, J., Fineberg, N., Msetfi, R. M., & Ball, L. J. (2007). Reasoning in People with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 397-411. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466507X228438
[32] Yilmaz, A. E., Gençöz, T., & Wells, A. (2008). Psychometric Characteristics of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire and Meta-Cognitions Questionannaire-30 and Meta-Cognitive Predictors of Worry and Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in a Turkish Sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 15, 424-439. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.589