情绪对两类限制性饮食者行为抑制控制的影响
The Effects of Emotion on Behavioral Inhibitory Control of Two Different Subgroups of Restrained Eaters
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2017.71016, PDF, HTML, XML, 下载: 1,968  浏览: 7,231 
作者: 袭玉荣, 陈 红:西南大学心理学部,重庆
关键词: 限制性饮食者情绪行为抑制控制Restrained Eater Emotion Behavioral Inhibitory Control
摘要: 限制性饮食者被认为是为了降低和维持体重而严格控制饮食的慢性节食人群。边界理论认为,限制性进食消耗大量认知资源。而情绪占用认知资源,导致解脱进食致使节食失败。情绪对成功和失败两类限制性饮食者行为抑制控制的影响尚未明确。本研究采用Go-Nogo范式分别探讨了阈上和阈下情绪对两类限制性饮食者抑制控制能力的影响。结果发现失败的限制性饮食者对食物线索的抑制控制能力异常地受负性情绪的干扰,成功的限制性饮食者对食物的抑制控制能力不受负性情绪的干扰,负性情绪对两类限制性饮食者影响的差异可能是限制性饮食成败的重要原因之一。
Abstract: Restrained eaters have been defined as chronic dieters who restrict their food intake to lose or keep their bodyweight. Boundary theory of eating suggests that restricting food consumption takes vast cognitive resources, so it may impact food consumption of restrained eaters while emotion occupies cognitive resources. However, the effects of emotion on inhibitory control of different subgroups of restrained eaters are still unclear. Present study is aimed to investigate the difference of inhibitory control by Go-Nogo paradigm between two different subgroups of restrained eaters under both threshold emotion and subliminal affective priming. The results show that the inhibitory control in response to food-cues of unsuccessful restrained eaters is particularly interfered by negative emotion. On the contrary, there is no such effect on successful restrained eaters. The different influence of negative emotion on different subgroups might be one of the most important factors for successful restrained eating.
文章引用:袭玉荣, 陈红 (2017). 情绪对两类限制性饮食者行为抑制控制的影响. 心理学进展, 7(1), 124-139. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.71016

参考文献

[1] 杜捷, 陈红, 苏艳华, 高笑, 孟景(2014). 自我调控资源对限制性饮食者进食的影响. 中国临床心理学杂志, 22, 1-6.
[2] 黄宇霞, 罗跃嘉(2004). 国际情绪图片系统在中国的试用研究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 18, 631-634.
[3] 孔繁昌, 张妍, 陈红, 石明丽, Jackson, T., 高笑(2011). 限制性饮食者对食物线索的认知偏向: 行为和脑机制的证据. 心理科学进展, 19, 1355-1362.
[4] 翁春燕, 陈红, 朱岚(2012). 限制性饮食者对食物线索的注意偏向: 基于目标矛盾理论模型. 心理学报, 44, 680-689.
[5] 辛勇, 李红, 袁加锦(2010). 负性情绪干扰行为抑制控制: 一项事件相关电位研究. 心理学报, 42, 334-341.
[6] Albert, J., Lópezmartín, S., & Carretié, L. (2010). Emotional Context Modulates Response Inhibition: Neural and Behavioral Data. NeuroImage, 49, 914-921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.045
[7] Baumgartner, T., Esslen, M., & Jäncke, L. (2006). From Emotion Perception to Emotion Experience: Emotions Evoked by Pictures and Classical Music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 34-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.04.007
[8] Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., & Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Source Analysis of the N2 in a Cued Go/NoGo Task. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 221-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011
[9] Bjorklund, D. F., & Shackelford, T. K. (1999). Differences in Parental Investment Contribute to Important Differences between Men and Women. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 86-89. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00020
[10] Blair, E. H., Wing, R. R., & Wald, A. (1991). The Effect of Laboratory Stressors on Glycemic Control and Gastrointestinal Transit Time. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 133-143. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-199103000-00003
[11] Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2007). Inhibition of Action, Thought, and Emotion: A Selective Neurobiological Review. Applied & Preventive Psychology Journal of the American Association of Applied & Preventive Psychology, 12, 99-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.004
[12] Dimoska, A., Johnstone, S. J., & Barry, R. J. (2006). The Audito-ry-Evoked N2 and P3 Components in the Stop-Signal Task: Indices of Inhibition, Response-Conflict or Error-Detection? Brain & Cognition, 62, 98-112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.011
[13] Donkers, F. C., & van Boxtel, G. J. (2004). The N2 in Go/No-Go Tasks Reflects Conflict Monitoring Not Response Inhibition. Brain & Cognition, 56, 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005
[14] Elliott, R., Rubinsztein, J. S., Sahakian, B. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Selective Attention to Emotional Stimuli in a Verbal Go/No-Go Task: An fMRI Study. Neuroreport, 11, 1739-1744. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200006050-00028
[15] Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive Frontal Functions. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 66-70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000401
[16] Greeno, C. G., & Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-Induced Eating. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 444-464. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444
[17] Hachl, P., Hempel, C., &Pietrowsky, R. (2003). ERPs to Stimulus Identification in Persons with Restrained Eating Behavior. International Journal of Psychophysiology Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 49, 111-121. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00099-0
[18] Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., &Polivy, J. (1991). Effects of Physical Threat and Ego Threat on Eating Behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 60, 138-143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.138
[19] Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and Unrestrained Eating. Journal of Personality, 43, 647-660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00727.x
[20] Izard, C. E., & Ackerman, B. P. (2000). Motivational, Organizational, and Regulatory Functions of Discrete Emotions. In M. Lewis, & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (2nd ed., pp. 253-322). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
[21] Keller, C., & Siegrist, M. (2014). Successful and Unsuccessful Restrained Eating. Does Dispositional Self-Control Matter? Appetite, 74, 101-106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.11.019
[22] Koball, A. M., Meers, M. R., Storferisser, A., Domoff, S. E., & Mushereizenman, D. R. (2012) Eating When Bored: Revision of the Emotional Eating Scale with a Focus on Boredom. Health Psychology, 31, 521-524.
[23] Kong, F., Yan, Z., & Hong, C. (2015). Inhibition Ability of Food Cues between Successful and Unsuccessful Restrained Eaters: A Two-Choice Oddball Task. PloS ONE, 10, e0120522. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120522
[24] Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). Psychophysiology of Musical Emotions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3-6.
[25] Li, C. S., Huang, C., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2006). Imaging Response Inhibition in a Stop-Signal Task: Neural Correlates Independent of Signal Monitoring and Post-Response Processing. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26, 186-192. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3741-05.2006
[26] Lowe, M. R. (1993). The Effects of Dieting on Eating Behavior: A Three-Factor Model. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 100-121. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.100
[27] Macht, M., & Simons, G. (2000). Emotions and Eating in Everyday Life. Appetite, 35, 65-71. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2000.0325
[28] Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, A.-M., Samuels, B., & Chatman, J. (2007). Medicare’s Search for Effective Obesity Treatments: Diets Are Not the Answer. The American psychologist, 62, 220-233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.220
[29] Mm, W. D., & Hetherington (2004). Stress and Eating: The Effects of Ego-Threat and Cognitive Demand on Food Intake in Restrained and Emotional Eaters. Appetite, 43, 39-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.02.001
[30] Nagy, E., Potts, G. F., & Loveland, K. A. (2003). Sex-Related ERP Differences in Deviance Detection. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48, 285-292. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00042-4
[31] Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T., Havermans, R. C., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Impulsivity in Obese Women. Appetite, 47, 253-256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.008
[32] Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Curtin, L. R., McDowell, M. A., Tabak, C. J., & Flegal, K. M. (2006). Prevalence of Overweight and Obesity in the United States, 1999-2004. JAMA, 295, 1549-1555. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.295.13.1549
[33] Oliver, G., & Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived Effects of Stress on Food Choice. Physiology & Behavior, 66, 511-515. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9
[34] Oliver, G., Wardle, J., & Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and Food Choice: A Laboratory Study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 853-865. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016
[35] Paz-Filho, G. J., & Licinio, J. (2009). Nervosa. Hormones, Brain and Behavior (pp. 2927-2943).
[36] Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B. (1998). Music and Emotion: Perceptual Determinants, Immediacy, and Isolation after Brain Damage. Cognition, 68, 111-141. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00043-2
[37] Polivy, J. (1976). Perception of Calories and Regulation of Intake in Restrained and Unrestrained Subjects. Addictive Behaviors, 1, 237-243. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(76)90016-2
[38] Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1999). Distress and Eating: Why Do Dieters Overeat? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 153-164. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199909)26:2<153::AID-EAT4>3.0.CO;2-R
[39] Rotenberg, K. J., & Flood, D. (1999). Loneliness, Dysphoria, Dietary Restraint, and Eating Behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 55-64. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199901)25:1<55::AID-EAT7>3.0.CO;2-#
[40] Ruderman, A. J. (1986). Dietary Restraint: A Theoretical and Empirical Review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 247-262. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.2.247
[41] Stice, E., Davis, K., Miller, N. P., & Marti, C. N. (2008). Fasting In-creases Risk for Onset of Binge Eating and Bulimic Pathology: A 5-Year Prospective Study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 941-946. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013644
[42] Stice, E., Sysko, R., Roberto, C. A., & Allison, S. (2009). Are Dietary Restraint Scales Valid Measures of Dietary Restriction? Additional Objective Behavioral and Biological Data Suggest Not. Appetite, 54, 331-339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.009
[43] Strien, T. V., & Ouwens, M. A. (2007). Effects of Distress, Alexithymia and Impulsivity on Eating. Eating Behaviors, 8, 251-257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.06.004
[44] Stroebe, W., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Papies, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2013). Why Most Dieters Fail but Some Succeed: A Goal Conflict Model of Eating Behavior. Psychological review, 120, 110-138. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030849
[45] Tepper, B. J., Trail, A. C., & Shaffer, S. E. (1996). Diet and Physical Activity in Restrained Eaters. Appetite, 27, 51-64. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1996.0033
[46] Thomas, J. G., Bond, D. S., Hill, J. O., & Wing, R. R. (2011). The National Weight Control Registry: A Study of “Successful Losers”. ACSM’s Health & Fitness Journal, 15, 8-12. https://doi.org/10.1249/FIT.0b013e31820b72b5
[47] Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of Processing in the Human Visual System. Nature, 381, 520. https://doi.org/10.1038/381520a0
[48] Van Blyderveen, S., Lafrance, A., Emond, M., Kosmerly, S., O’Connor, M., & Chang, F. (2016). Personality Differences in the Susceptibility to Stress-Eating: The Influence of Emotional Control and Impulsivity. Eating Behaviors, 23, 76-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.07.009
[49] Veen, V., & Carter, C. (2002). The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593-602. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290260045837
[50] Wadden, T. A., Brownell, K. D., & Foster, G. D. (2002). Obesity: Responding to the Global Epidemic. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 70, 510-525. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.3.510
[51] Wang, Y., Yang, J., Yuan, J., Fu, A., Meng, X., & Li, H. (2011). The Impact of Emotion Valence on Brain Processing of Behavioral Inhibitory Control: Spatiotemporal Dynamics. Neuroscience Letters, 502, 112-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.07.039
[52] We, J. H., & Luo, Y. J. (2002). Event-Related Brain Potentials: The Cognitive ERP Textbook.
[53] Willner, P., & Healy, S. (1994). Decreased Hedonic Responsiveness during a Brief Depressive Mood Swing. Journal of Affective Disorders, 32, 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(94)90056-6
[54] Wing, R. R., Blair, E. H., Epstein, L. H., & Mcdermott, M. D. (1990). Psychological Stress and Glucose Metabolism in Obese and Normal-Weight Subjects: A Possible Mechanism for Differences in Stress-Induced Eating. Health Psychology Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology American Psychological Association, 9, 693-700. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.9.6.693
[55] Yuan, J. J., Yang, J. M., Meng, X. X., Yu, F. Q., & Li, H. (2008). The Valence Strength of Negative Stimuli Modulates Visual Novelty Processing: Electrophysiological Evidence from an Event-Related Potential Study. Neuroscience, 157, 524-531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.09.023
[56] Yuan, J., He, Y., Qinglin, Z., Chen, A., & Li, H. (2008). Gender Differences in Behavioral Inhibitory Control: ERP Evidence from a Two-Choice Oddball Task. Psychophysiology, 45, 986-993. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00693.x
[57] Yuan, J., Zhang, Q., Chen, A., Li, H., Wang, Q., Zhuang, Z., & Jia, S. (2007). Are We Sensitive to Valence Differences in Emotionally Negative Stimuli? Electrophysiological Evidence from an ERP Study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2764-2771. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.018