回避型依恋者对消极情绪信息防御性加工特征
The Defensive Characteristics of Negative Emotional Information Processing of Avoidant Attachment
摘要: 准确加工情绪信息在人际关系管理及一般社会功能上起重要作用。不安全依恋风格影响个体的情绪加工偏向。回避型依恋者抑制消极刺激的加工,并且被看做拥有早期无意识的焦虑自我模型以及晚期控制的积极自我模型。本文主要探讨回避型依恋者加工消极情绪信息的防御性排斥特征,具体表现为一种早期警觉和晚期抑制的双重加工模型。行为证据证明了回避型依恋者在早期自动化加工阶段表现出同依恋焦虑者类似的对威胁相关信息的警觉探测。ERP成分也体现了回避者早期警觉的特征,表现在N1、P2等注意警觉相关成分以及C1、P1等基于粗略视觉特征的视觉情绪刺激加工相关的早成分更为强烈的波幅上。在晚期加工阶段这类个体则表现出对消极情绪的回避抑制加工,在表情模仿任务中出现“反转的微笑”以及stroop任务中更少的干扰效应证明其在晚期加工阶段的抑制加工特征。神经层面表现为回避型个体在认知控制脑区如背外侧前额叶、眶额皮层等脑区的激活。回避者对负性情绪的高度警觉是成功回避的前提,而这一策略在个体认知资源不足时出现失效。未来研究可进一步探讨回避型依恋与警觉-抑制注意模式的因果基础;关注防御性情绪信息加工对个体健康以及关系维持的影响及后果;深入考察去激活策略功能的有效性及其神经机制;深化回避防御在临床治疗上的应用,进一步为治疗依恋混乱相关的临床策略提供新的启示。
Abstract: Accurate processing of emotional information plays an important role in the management of in-terpersonal relationship and social function. Insecure attachment style effects the process of threaten related information. Individuals with avoidant pattern of attachment are assumed to repress threaten-related signals. They are assumed to have one internal working model associated with anxiety, operating out of awareness at early, automatic stages of information processing, and another positive model operating at later, cognitively controlled stages of processing. This paper discussed the defensive characteristics of negative emotion processing of avoidant attachment. Automatic vigilance followed by effortful inhibition as a cognitive defense for avoidant attachment to processing the threaten-related signals, which is expressed in a specific dual-process model of avoidant defenses included vigilance in early automatic process and avoidance in later controlled process. The evidences from various behavioral experimental paradigms included emoticon mimicry task, morph task, perceptual threshold task, as well as emotional cueing task provided support for the point of view that attachment avoidant individuals have the capacity to detect the threaten related information in certain circumstance in the early stage of information processing. This characteristic was also proved by the evidences from event-related potentials (ERP) components, which are mainly reflected in more strong amplitude in attention vigilance related components (N1, P2) and early components related to visual features-based emotion processing when presented negative emotion information in attachment avoidant individuals. However, individuals with avoidant attachment style processed negative emotion with attention inhibition at the cognitively controlled level of processing. The avoidant individual’ tendency to “smiling” in response to the angry face at the controlled level was interpreted as a repression of their earlier, automatically evoked negative emotional reaction. Coincidentally, consistent with the proposal that avoidant individuals limit attention to potentially distressing information, attachment avoidance was associated with reductions in emotional Stroop (ES) interference for threatening related stimulus. These individuals exhibited limited cognitive resources which were devoted in this phase. The evidences from functional magnetic resonance imaging showed that during the processing of negative emotion, these individuals with attachment avoidant exhibited more strong activities in brain region in charge of advanced cognitive and control function, such as the right hemisphere superior frontal gyms (rSFG), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the orbital frontal cortex (OFC) and so on. It can be explained that effectively avoiding processing threaten related information is based on the heightened state of vigilance for attachment avoidance. The defense strategy of attachment avoidance was considered as a secondary strategy with cognitive effort to sustain in later stage of emotion information processing. This characteristic of cognitive resource consumption made the strategy ineffective when the cognitive resource deficit. Future studies can further explore the causal basis for the relationship between avoidant attachment and vigilance-inhibi- tion attention pattern, pay attention to the impact of defensive emotional processing on individual health and the maintain of intimate relationship, explore the neural mechanism and effectiveness of the deactivation strategy and further study the application of avoid defense in clinical treatment to provide new inspiration for the treatment of attachment disorder and related clinical strategies.
文章引用:唐清婷, 胡佳, 陈旭, 刘颖 (2017). 回避型依恋者对消极情绪信息防御性加工特征. 心理学进展, 7(2), 199-206. https://doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.72026

参考文献

[1] 马书采, 丁玲, 田志霄, 杨晓莉, 周爱保, 赵明仁(2013). 成人去激活依恋策略和注意偏向: 来自表情图片外源性提示点探测任务的证据. 心理科学, 36(3), 592-599.
[2] Andriopoulos, P., & Kafetsios, K. (2015). Avoidant Attachment and the Processing of Emotion Information: Selective Attention or Cognitive Avoidance? Journal of Relationships Research, 6, e6. https://doi.org/10.1017/jrr.2015.2
[3] Avero, P., Corace, K. M., Endler, N. S., & Calvo, M. G. (2003). Coping Styles and Threat Processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 843-861. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00287-8
[4] Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of Intimacy: An Attachment Perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407590072001
[5] Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual Processing during the conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 80-93. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563265
[6] Calvo, M. G., & Eysenck, M. W. (2000). Early Vigilance and Late Avoidance of Threat Processing: Repressive Coping versus Low/High Anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 763-787. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930050156627
[7] Chris Fraley, R., Niedenthal, P. M., Marks, M., Brumbaugh, C., & Vicary, A. (2006). Adult Attachment and the Perception of Emotional Expressions: Probing the Hyperactivating Strategies Underlying Anxious Attachment. Journal of Personality, 74, 1163-1190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00406.x
[8] Chun, D. S., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., Mathews, A., & Jorgensen, T. D. (2015). Testing a Dual-Process Model of Avoidant Defenses. Journal of Research in Personality, 55, 75-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.02.002
[9] Derakshan, N., Eysenck, M. W., & Myers, L. B. (2007). Emotional Information Processing in Repressors: The Vigilance-Avoidance Theory. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1585-1614. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701499857
[10] Dewitte, M. (2011). Adult Attachment and Attentional Inhibition of Interpersonal Stimuli. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 612-625. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.508683
[11] Edelstein, R. S. (2006). Attachment and Emotional Memory: Investigating the Source and Extent of Avoidant Memory Impairments. Emotion, 6, 340. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.340
[12] Edelstein, R. S., & Gillath, O. (2008). Avoiding Interference: Adult Attachment and Emotional Processing Biases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 171-181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207310024
[13] Eimer, M., & Holmes, A. (2007). Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates of Emotional Face Processing. Neuropsychologia, 45, 15-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.022
[14] Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2011). Attachment Insecurities and the Processing of Threat-Related Information: Studying the Schemas Involved in Insecure People’s Coping Strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022503
[15] Fraedrich, E. M., Lakatos, K., & Spangler, G. (2010). Brain Activity during Emotion Perception: The Role of Attachment Representation. Attachment & Human Development, 12, 231-248. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616731003759724
[16] Fraley, R. C., & Waller, N. G. (1998). Adult Attachment Patterns: A Test of the Typological Model. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment Theory and Close Relationships (pp. 77-114). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
[17] Fraley, R. C., Garner, J. P., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult Attachment and the Defensive Regulation of Attention and Memory: Examining the Role of Preemptive and Postemptive Defensive Processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 816. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.816
[18] Gillath, O., Giesbrecht, B., & Shaver, P. R. (2009). Attachment, Attention, and Cognitive Control: Attachment Style and Performance on General Attention Tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 647-654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.011
[19] Halit, H., Csibra, G., Volein, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Face-Sensitive Cortical Processing in Early Infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1228-1234. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00321.x
[20] Hester, R., Murphy, K., Foxe, J. J., Foxe, D. M., Javitt, D. C., & Garavan, H. (2004). Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 776-785. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904970726
[21] Kohn, J. L., Rholes, W. S., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). Self-Regulatory Depletion and Attachment Avoidance: Increasing the Accessibility of Negative Attachment-Related Memories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 375-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.020
[22] Le, B., Dove, N. L., Agnew, C. R., Korn, M. S., & Mutso, A. A. (2010). Predicting Nonmarital Romantic Relationship Dissolution: A Meta-Analytic Synthesis. Personal Relationships, 17, 377-390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01285.x
[23] MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive Bias Modification Ap-proaches to Anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189-217. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052
[24] Maier, M. A., Bernier, A., Pekrun, R., Zimmermann, P., Strasser, K., & Grossmann, K. E. (2005). Attachment State of Mind and Perceptual Processing of Emotional Stimuli. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 67-81. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500039606
[25] Marks, M. J., & Vicary, A. M. (2015). The Interplay and Effectiveness of Implicit and Explicit Avoidant Defenses. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 33, 619-639.
[26] Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houde, O., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. (2001). Cortical Networks for Working Memory and Executive Functions Sustain the Conscious Resting State in Man. Brain Research Bulletin, 54, 287-298. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00437-8
[27] Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The Attachment Behavioral System in Adulthood: Activation, Psychodynamics, and Interpersonal Processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 53-152.
[28] Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
[29] Mikulincer, M., Dolev, T., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Attachment-Related Strategies during Thought Suppression: Ironic Rebounds and Vulnerable Self-Representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 940. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.940
[30] Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the Attachment System in Adulthood: Threat-Related Primes Increase the Accessibility of Mental Representations of Attachment Figures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 881. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.881
[31] Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Robin, L., & Innes-Ker, Å. H. (2002). Adult Attachment and the Perception of Facial Expression of Emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 419. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.419
[32] Rellecke, J., Palazova, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). On the Automaticity of Emotion Processing in Words and Faces: Event-Related Brain Potentials Evidence from a Superficial Task. Brain and Cognition, 77, 23-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.001
[33] Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-Related Psychodynamics. Attachment & Human Development, 4, 133-161. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730210154171
[34] Simpson, J. A., & Steven Rholes, W. (2012). 6 Adult Attachment Orientations, Stress, and Romantic Relationships. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 279. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394286-9.00006-8
[35] Sonnby-Borgström, M., & Jönsson, P. (2004). Dismissing-Avoidant Pattern of Attachment and Mimicry Reactions at Different Levels of Information Processing. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 103-113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2004.00385.x
[36] Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult Attachment Predicts Maternal Brain and Oxytocin Response to Infant Cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 2655-2666. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.103
[37] Tan, R., Overall, N. C., & Taylor, J. K. (2012). Let’s Talk about Us: Attachment, Relationship-Focused Disclosure, and Relationship Quality. Personal Relationships, 19, 521-534. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01383.x
[38] Van Kleef, G. A. (2010). The Emerging View of Emotion as Social Information. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 331-343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00262.x
[39] Vrtička, P., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Influence of Adult Attachment Style on the Perception of Social and Non-Social Emotional Scenes. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 29, 530-544. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512443451
[40] Yee, C. I., & Shiota, M. N. (2015). An Insecure Base: Attachment Style and Orienting Response to Positive Stimuli. Psychophysiology, 52, 905-909. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12422
[41] Zhang, X., Li, T., & Zhou, X. (2008). Brain Responses to Facial Expressions by Adults with Different Attachment-Orientations. Neuroreport, 19, 437-441. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f55728