简单形状的情绪效应
The Affective Effect of Simple Geometric Shapes
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2015.58061, PDF, HTML, XML,  被引量 下载: 4,832  浏览: 22,454 
作者: 王依楠, 张 钦:首都师范大学,北京市认知与学习重点实验室,北京
关键词: 简单形状情绪意义威胁刺激情绪启动范式Simple Shapes Affective Meaning Threaten Stimulus Affective Priming Paradigm
摘要: 本研究采用情绪启动范式,以首都师范大学在校生为被试,检验了简单形状的情绪效应。实验一发现快乐表情的情绪一致性效应,圆形启动刺激促进对快乐表情的判断,倒三角形启动刺激干扰表情判断。实验二发现形状的情绪一致性影响,倒三角形促进对消极词语的加工,干扰对积极词语的加工,圆形产生相反的作用。实验表明,圆形表征着积极意义,倒三角形表征消极意义,且这种情绪意义可以被自动激活,对认知操作产生不同的影响。
Abstract: Selecting undergraduates of Capital Normal University as participants, the present work sought to examine the emotional meaning of simple geometric shapes with an affective priming paradigm. In Experiment 1, we found an effect of affective congruency on happy faces. Results showed that the happy faces were recognized faster after circle than inverted triangle primes. Experiment 2 revealed a significant effect of affective congruency. The inverted triangle facilitated the processing of negative words and interfered with the processing of positive words. The shape of circle had an opposite effect. The present findings support that inverted triangles represent an unpleasant or threatening meaning, whereas circles are perceived as pleasant stimuli. This meaning could be ac-tivated automatically and exert different influences on cognitive performance.
文章引用:王依楠, 张钦 (2015). 简单形状的情绪效应. 心理学进展, 5(8), 471-480. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2015.58061

参考文献

[1] 罗跃嘉, 王一牛(2004). 汉语情绪词系统(CAWS). 北京: 中国科学院心理学研究所.
[2] 王妍, 罗跃嘉(2005). 大学生面孔表情材料的标准化及评定. 中国临床心理学杂志, 4期, 396-398.
[3] Armbruster, D., Suchert, V., Gärtner, A., & Strobel, A. (2014). Threatening shapes: The impact of simple geometric configurations on peripheral physiological markers. Physiology & Behavior, 135, 215-221.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.06.020
[4] Aronoff, J. (2006). How we recognize angry and happy emotion in people, places, and things. Cross-Cultural Research, 40, 83-105.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397105282597
[5] Aronoff, J., Barclay, A. M., & Stevenson, L. A. (1988). The rec-ognition of threatening facial stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 647-655.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.647
[6] Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configural bases of emotion recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1050-1066.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.1050
[7] Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychological Science, 17, 645-648.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.x
[8] Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2007). Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2191-2200.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.008
[9] Bassili, J. N. (1978). Facial motion in the perception of faces and of emotional expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 373-379.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.4.3.373
[10] Britton, J. C., Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F. , Welsh, R. C., Berridge, K. C., & Liberzon, I. (2006). Neural correlates of social and nonsocial emotions: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 31, 397-409.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.027
[11] Castner, J. E., Chenery, H. J., Copland, D. A., Coyne, T. J., Sinclair, F., & Silburn, P. A. (2007). Semantic and affective priming as a function of stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 130, 1395-1407.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm059
[12] Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229-238.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
[13] Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Jelsone, L. M., Nathan, P. J., & Phan, K. L. (2006). Beyond threat: Amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. Neuroimage, 30, 1441-1448.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.003
[14] Hietanen, J. K., & Astikainen, P. (2013). N170 response to facial expressions is modulated by the affective congruency between the emotional expression and preceding affective picture. Biological Psychology, 92, 114-124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.005
[15] Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2001). A time courseanalysis of the affective priming effect. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 143-165.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930125768
[16] Hermans, D., Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2003). Affec-tive priming with subliminally presented pictures. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 97-114.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087416
[17] Klauer, K. C., Rossnagel, C., & Musch, J. (1997). List-context effects in evaluative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 246-255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.246
[18] Kring, A. M., & Gordon, A. H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: Expression, experience, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 686-703.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.686
[19] Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., Sarinopoulos, I. C., & Zhu, D. C. (2009). Recognizing threat: Simple geometric shapes activate neural circuitry underlying threat detection. Journal of Cog-nitive Neuroscience, 21, 1523-1535.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21111
[20] Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Stearns, J. (2007). The shape of threat: Simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion, 7, 526-534.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.526
[21] Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Steuer, E. L. (2012). Simple geometric shapes are implicitly associated with affective value. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 404-413.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9249-2
[22] LoBue, V., & Larson, C. L. (2010). What makes an angry face look so angry? Examining the shape of threat in children and adults. Visual Cognition, 18, 1165-1178.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13506281003783675
[23] Lundqvist, D., Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (1999). The face of wrath: Critical features for conveying facial threat. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 691-711.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026999399379041
[24] Lundqvist, D., Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (2004). The face of wrath: The role of features and configurations in conveying social threat. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 161-182.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000453
[25] Lundqvist, D., & Ohman, A. (2005). Emotion regulates attention: The relation between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual attention. Visual Cognition, 12, 51-84.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000085
[26] Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
[27] Sternglanz, S. H., Gray, J. L., & Murakami, M. (1977). Adult preferences for infantile facial features: An ethological approach. Animal Behavior, 25, 108-115.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(77)90072-0
[28] Toet, A., & Tak, S. (2013). Look out, there is a triangle behind you! The effect of primitive geometric shapes on perceived facial dominance. I-Perception, 4, 53-56.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0568sas
[29] Tipples, J., Atkinson, A. P., & Young, A. W. (2002). The eyebrow frown: A salient social signal. Emotion, 2, 288-296.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.2.3.288
[30] Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 624-631.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1057
[31] Watson, D. G., Blagrove, E., Evans, C., & Moore, L. (2012). Negative triangles: Simple geometric shapes convey emotional valence. Emotion, 12, 18-22.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024495
[32] Watson, D. G., Blagrove, E., & Selwood, S. (2011). Emotional triangles: A test of emotion-based attentional capture by simple geometric shapes. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1149-1164.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.525861
[33] Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S. et al. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science, 306, 2061.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1103617
[34] Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
[35] Zhang, Q., Li, X. H., Gold, B. T., & Jiang, Y. (2010). Neural correlates of cross-domain affective priming. Brain Research, 1329, 142-151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.021