具身数量:感觉运动和身体运动对空间数量表征的影响
Embodied Numerosity: Sensori-Motor and Body Movement Influence Spatial-Numerical Representation
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2016.610140, PDF, HTML, XML,  被引量 下载: 2,157  浏览: 2,628  科研立项经费支持
作者: 朱荣娟, 周 圆, 张 宇, 游旭群:陕西师范大学心理学院,陕西省行为与认知神经科学重点实验室,陕西 西安
关键词: SNAs效应感觉运动身体运动手指计数具身认知SNAs Effect Sensori-Motor Body Movement Finger Counting Embodied Cognition
摘要: 空间数量联结效应(Spatial-Numerical Associations, SNAs)是指包括阿拉伯数字在内的形式更广泛的数量与空间之间的联结效应。本文从感觉和身体运动的角度,详细概述了视觉系统和前庭系统,身体运动和手指计数对空间数量表征的影响,并用具身理论对其进行新的解释,认为数量认知源于感觉和身体经验。未来的研究需借助于脑成像等研究手段来深入探讨基于感觉和身体运动的空间数量表征的神经生理机制,以及从纵向发展角度考查手指计数活动与空间数量表征之间的本质联系。
Abstract: The SNAs effect refers to the association effect between space and a wide range of quantities in-cluding Arabic numerals. In this paper, the effects of visual system and vestibular system, body motion and finger counting on SNAs are described in detail from the perspective of sensation and body movement. A new explanation is given with the theory of embodiment in this paper indicating that the cognition of numbers origins from the senses and physical experiences. Future research needs to explore the neurophysiological mechanism of SNAs based on sensory and body motion by means of brain imaging and other research methods, and examine the essential relationship between finger counting activity and SNAs from the perspective of longitudinal development.
文章引用:朱荣娟, 周圆, 张宇, 游旭群 (2016). 具身数量:感觉运动和身体运动对空间数量表征的影响. 心理学进展, 6(10), 1108-1116. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2016.610140

参考文献

[1] 陈巍, 汪寅(2013). 独水上的桥梁:镜像神经元与教育实践——兼对“镜像教育”的批判. 全球教育展望, 42(1), 74-85.
[2] 李其维(2008). “认知革命”与“第二代认知科学”刍议. 心理学报, 40(12), 1306-1327.
[3] 王一峰, 张丽, 刘春雷, 李红(2010). 空间量化的心理表征. 心理科学进展, 18(4), 560-568.
[4] 叶浩生(2010). 具身认知: 认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18(5), 705-710.
[5] 叶浩生(2014). 具身涵义的理论辨析.心理学报, 46(7), 1032-1042.
[6] 叶浩生(2012). 具身认知、镜像神经元与身心关系. 广州大学学报(社会科学版), 11(3), 32-36.
[7] Andres, M., Michaux, N., & Pesenti, M. (2012). Common Substrate for Mental Arithmetic and Finger Repre-sentation in the Parietal Cortex. Neuroimage, 62, 1520-1528.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.047
[8] Andres, M., Olivier, E., & Badets, A. (2008). Actions, Words, and Numbers. A Motor Contribution to Semantic Processing? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 313-317.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00597.x
[9] Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2010). Creating Number Semantics through Finger Movement Perception. Cognition, 115, 46-53.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.007
[10] Badets, A., Bouquet, C. A., Ric, F., & Pesenti, M. (2012). Number Generation Bias after Action Observation. Experimental brain research, 221, 43-49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3145-1
[11] Badets, A., Boutin, A., & Heuer, H. (2015). Mental Representa-tions of Magnitude and Order: A Dissociation by Sensorimotor Learning. Acta Psychologica, 157, 164-175.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.03.004
[12] Chen, Y. H., Zhou, J. F., & Yeh, S. L. (2015). Beyond the SNARC Effect: Distance-Number Mapping Occurs in the Peripersonal Space. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 1519-1528.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4225-9
[13] Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror Neurons: From Origin to Function. Behavioral and Brain Science, 37, 177-192.
[14] Crollen, V., Dormala, G., Seron, X., Lepore, F., & Collignon, O. (2013). Embodied Number: The Role of Vision in the Development of Number-Space Interactions. Cortex, 49, 276-283.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.11.006
[15] Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
[16] Di Luca, S., Grana, A., Semenza, C., Seron, X., & Pesenti, M. (2006). Finger-Digit Compatibility in Arabic Numeral Processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1648-1663.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210500256839
[17] Di Luca, S., Lefevre, N., & Pesenti, M. (2010). Place and Sum-mation Coding for Canonical and Non-Canonical Finger Numeral Representations. Cognition, 117, 95-100.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.008
[18] Domahs, F., Moller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2010)Embodied Numerosity: Implicit Hand-Based Representations Influence Symbolic Number Processing across Cultures. Cognition, 116, 251-266.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007
[19] Eerland, A., Guadalupe, T., & Zwaan, R. (2011). Leaning to the Left Makes the Eiffel Tower Seem Smaller: Posture- Modulated Estimation. Psychological Science, 22, 1511-1514.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611420731
[20] Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger Counting Habits Modulate Spa-tial-Numerical Associations. Cortex, 44, 386-392.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.004
[21] Fischer, M. H. (2012). A Hierarchical View of Grounded, Em-bodied, and Situated Numerical Cognition. Cognitive Process, 13, S161-S164.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0477-5
[22] Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When Digits Help Digits: Spatial-Numerical Associations Point to Finger Counting as Prime Example of Embodied Cognition. Frontiers in Psy-chology Cognition, 2, 260.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
[23] Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2010). How to Cook a SNARC: Number Placement in Text Rapidly Changes Spatial-Numerical Associations. Brain and Cognition, 72, 333-336.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010
[24] Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-Motor Spatial Training of Number Magnitude Representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 177-183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3
[25] Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593
[26] Galton, F. (1880). Visualised Numerals. Nature, 21, 252-256.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/021252a0
[27] Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences Is Spatially Organized. Cognition, 87, B87-B95.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00234-2
[28] Gherri, E., & Eimer, M. (2010). Manual Response Preparation Disrupts Spatial Attention: An Electrophysiological Investigation of Links between Action and Attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 961-969.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.017
[29] Hartmann, M, Grabherr, L., & Mast, F. W. (2011). Moving along the Mental Number Line: Interactions between Whole-Body Motion and Numerical Cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1416-1427.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026706
[30] Hartmann, M., Farkas, R., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Self-Motion Perception Influences Number Processing: Evidence from a Parity Task. Cognitive Processing, 13, S189-S192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0484-6
[31] Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A Framework for Perception and Action Planning. The Behavioral and Brain Science, 24, 849-878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000103
[32] Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between Number and Space in Parietal Cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435-448.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1684
[33] Kaufmann, L, Vogel, S. E, Wood, G., Kremser, C., Schocke, M., Zimmerhackl, L. B., & Koten, J. W. (2008). A Developmental fMRI Study of Nonsymbolic Numerical and Spatial Processing. Cortex, 44, 376-385.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.003
[34] Kilner, J. M., & Lemon, R. N. (2013). What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons. Current Biology, 23, R1057-R1062.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.051
[35] Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger Counting Habits in Middle Eastern and Western Individuals: An Online Survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 566-578.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022111406254
[36] Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye Position Predicts What Number You Have in Mind. Current Biology, 20, R264-R265.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.015
[37] Loetscher, T., Schwarz, U., Schubiger, M., & Brugger, P. (2008). Head Turns Bias the Brain’s Internal Random Generator. Current Biology, 18, R60-R62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.015
[38] Moeller, K., Fischer, U., Link, T., Wasner, M., Huber, S., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2012). Learning and Development of Embodied Numerosity. Cognitive Processing, 13, S271-S274.
[39] Nieder, A. (2005). Counting on Neurons: The Neurobiology of Numerical Competence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 177-190.
[40] Patro, K., & Haman, M. (2012). The Spatial-Numerical Congruity Effect in Preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 534-542.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.09.006
[41] Penner-Wilger, M., Fast, L., LeFevre, J., Smith-Chant, B. L., Skwarchuk, S., Kamawar, D. et al. (2009). Subitizing, Finger Gnosis, and the Representation of Number. In: Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 520-525).
[42] Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2007). A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex. Neuron, 53, 293-305.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022
[43] Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., & Le-Bihan, D. (2001). Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task. NeuroImage, 14, 1013-1026.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0913
[44] Pitt, B., & Casasanto, D. (2014). Experiential Origins of the Mental Number Line. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M, McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Processing of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
[45] Riello, M., & Rusconi, E. (2011). Unimanual SNARC Effect: Hand Matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 372.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00372
[46] Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The Mirror Neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
[47] Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Vallinin, B., & Regoli, L. (2011). Asymmetrical Number-Space Mapping in the Avian Brain. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95, 231-238.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.012
[48] Rusconi, E., Walsh, V., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Dexterity with Numbers: rTMS over Left Angular Gyrus Disrupts Finger Gnosis and Number Processing. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1609-1624.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.009
[49] Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the Eyes along the Mental Number Line: Comparing SNARC Effects with Saccadic and Manual Responses. Perception & Psycho-physics, 66, 651-664.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194909
[50] Shaki, S., Petrusic, W. M., & Leth-Steensen, C. (2012). SNARC Effects with Numerical and Non-Numerical Symbolic Comparative Judgments: Instructional and Cultural Dependencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 515-530.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026729
[51] Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermuller, F. (2012). You Can Count on the Motor Cortex: Finger Counting Habits Modulate Motor Cortex Activation Evoked by Numbers. Neu-roImage, 59, 3139-3148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.037
[52] Wood, G., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Numbers, Space, and Action—From Finger Counting to the Mental Number Line and Beyond. Cortex, 44, 353-358.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.002
[53] Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed Hands and the SNARC Effect: A Failure to Replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42, 1069-1079.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70219-3
[54] Yang, T., Chen, C., Zhou, X., Xu, J., Dong, Q., & Chen, C. (2014). Development of Spatial Representation of Numbers: A Study of the SNARC Effect in Chinese Children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 1-11.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.011