高焦虑个体对情绪面孔的无意识注意偏向
The Unconscious Attention Bias to Emotional Faces in High-Trait-Anxiety Individuals
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2017.79146, PDF, HTML, XML, 下载: 1,844  浏览: 2,462  国家自然科学基金支持
作者: 张 爽, 杨洁莹, 崔丽霞:首都师范大学心理系,北京市“学习与认知”重点实验室,北京
关键词: 特质焦虑情绪面孔认知偏向阈下启动效应Trait Anxiety Emotional Faces Cognitive Bias Subliminal Priming Effect
摘要: 本研究采用中国化面孔情绪图片作刺激材料,通过阈下情绪启动任务比较高低特质焦虑个体对不同情绪面孔效价判断的正确率的差异。探讨高、低特质焦虑个体对正负性信息的无意识注意偏向的差异。结果发现:1) 呈现正性图片时,高焦虑个体在非启动条件下的正确率显著高于启动条件和控制条件。低焦虑组没有显著差异;2) 呈现负性图片时,高低焦虑个体在启动和控制条件下的正确率显著高于非启动条件;3) 在启动条件下,高焦虑个体对负性图片判断的正确率远高于正性图片,低焦虑个体没有显著差异;4) 在控制条件下,高低焦虑个体对负性目标图片判断的正确率显著高于正性目标图片。结论:高焦虑个体对威胁面孔存在无意识注意偏向。人们对恐惧情绪的反应强于其它表情。
Abstract: The study was designed to investigate the differences in the unconscious attention bias to positive and negative information in high trait anxiety and low trait anxiety individuals. Face pictures taken from Chinese Affective Picture System of Faces were used as stimuli. High trait anxiety (HTA) and low trait anxiety (LTA) individuals were tested with a subliminal emotional priming paradigm to examine the correct rates of judgments of different emotional faces. Results: 1) When presented the positive pictures, the hits rate in non-priming condition was significantly higher than in priming and control condition in HTA individuals, while there were no differences in LTA individuals; 2) When presented the negative pictures, the hits rate in priming and control condition was significantly higher than in non-priming condition in both HTA and LTA individuals; 3) Under subliminal priming condition, the hits rate of negative pictures was significantly higher than the positive pictures in HTA individuals, while there were no differences in LTA individuals; 4) Under subliminal control condition, the hits rate of negative pictures was significantly higher than the positive pictures in both HTA and LTA individuals. Conclusion: The unconscious processing of threat information of HTA individuals enhanced the conscious processing of threat information. Another conclusion of this study was that compared with other expressions, people were more sensitive to fear.
文章引用:张爽, 杨洁莹, 崔丽霞 (2017). 高焦虑个体对情绪面孔的无意识注意偏向. 心理学进展, 7(9), 1172-1179. https://doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.79146

参考文献

[1] 崔丽霞, 杨洁莹, 史光远, 张钦, 方平(2013). 非自主闯入焦虑发生的机制: 阈下启动效应的实验研究. 心理科学, 6, 1301-1306.
[2] 黄宇霞, 罗跃嘉(2004). 国际情绪图片系统在中国的试用研究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 18(9), 631-634.
[3] 吕创, 牛青云, 张学民(2014). 焦虑个体对负性刺激的注意偏向特点. 中国心理卫生杂志, 28(3), 208-214.
[4] 杨鹏, 蒋婧琪, 李松蔚, 姚泥沙, 余红玉, 王小玲, 等(2015). 社交焦虑大学生对正性评价解释偏向的初步探究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 29(12), 933-938.
[5] 姚泥沙, 李松蔚, 钱铭怡, 余红玉, 林沐雨, 王小玲, 等(2013). 高社交焦虑个体对正性面孔的情绪启动效应. 心理科学, 5, 1106-1109.
[6] 叶彬, 马佳瑶, 王璐, 洑佳玉, 余华萍, 胡琬琰, 等(2016). 非临床社交焦虑者对面部表情的自动加工机制. 中国临床心理学杂志, 6, 985-987.
[7] 蚁金瑶, 钟明天, 凌宇, 罗英姿, 姚树桥(2007). 情绪启动效应的时程特征. 中华行为医学与脑科学杂志, 16(3), 258-260.
[8] 袁承杰, 于凯, 刘伟志, 尚志蕾(2015). 青年人情绪启动差异性与注意偏向的实验研究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 5, 378-382.
[9] Barhaim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermanskranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-Related Attentional Bias in Anxious and Nonanxious Individuals: A Meta-Analytic Study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
[10] Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early Processing of the Six Basic Facial Emotional Expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613-620.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00174-5
[11] Carlson, J. M., Cha, J., & Mujicaparodi, L. R. (2013). Functional and Structural Amygdala - Anterior Cingulate Connectivity Correlates with Attentional Bias to Masked Fearful Faces. Cortex, 49, 2595-2600.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.008
[12] Dodd, H. F., Vogt, J., Turkileri, N., & Notebaert, L. (2016). Task Relevance of Emotional Information Affects Anxiety-Linked Attention Bias in Visual Search. Biological Psychology, 122, 13-20.
[13] Mcgrath, L. M., Oates, J. M., Dai, Y. G., Dodd, H. F., Waxler, J., Clements, C. C., et al. (2016). Attention Bias to Emotional Faces Varies by IQ and Anxiety in Williams Syndrome. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 46, 2174-2185.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2748-y
[14] Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., De Bono, J., & Painter, M. (1997). Time Course of Attentional Bias for Threat Information in Non-Clinical Anxiety. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 35, 297-303.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00109-X
[15] Paul, E. S., Pope, S. A., Fennell, J. G., & Mendl, M. T. (2012). Social Anxiety Modulates Subliminal Affective Priming. Plos One, 7, e37011.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0037011
[16] Schultz, L. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Attentional Focus in Social Anxiety Disorder: Potential for Interactive Processes. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1206-1221.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.04.003
[17] Sheppes, G., Luria, R., Fukuda, K., & Gross, J. J. (2013). There’s More to Anxiety than Meets the Eye: Isolating Threat-Related Attentional Engagement and Disengagement Biases. Emotion, 13, 520-528.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031236
[18] Sweeny, T. D., Grabowecky, M., Suzuki, S., & Paller, K. A. (2009). Long-Lasting Effects of Subliminal Affective Priming from Facial Expressions. Consciousness & Cognition, 18, 929-938.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.011
[19] Vizueta, N., Patrick, C. J., Yi, J., Thomas, K. M., & Sheng, H. (2011). Dispositional Fear, Negative Affectivity, and Neuroimaging Response to Visually Suppressed Emotional Faces. Neuroimage, 59, 761-771.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.015
[20] Weeks, J. W., Heimberg, R. G., Rodebaugh, T. L., & Norton, P. J. (2008). Exploring the Relationship between Fear of Positive Evaluation and Social Anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 386-400.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.04.009