限制性饮食者的自我调节能力
Restrained Eaters’ Self-Regulation Ability
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2017.711171, PDF, HTML, XML,  被引量 下载: 995  浏览: 1,469  科研立项经费支持
作者: 王维, 王密:西华师范大学教师教育学院,四川 南充;陈红, 周一舟:西南大学心理学部,重庆
关键词: 限制性饮食者自我调节能力侧抑制范式Restrained Eaters Self-Regulatory Ability Flanker Task
摘要: 研究考察了限制性饮食者的自我调节能力。方法:共87名女大学生被试(45限制性饮食者,42非限制性饮食者)参与了研究,实验采用侧抑制范式,包括目标刺激和分心刺激,均由高、低能量食物图片组成,要求被试注意目标刺激,忽视分心刺激。结果:1) 对于非限制性饮食者而言,目标主效应显著,其对高能量的目标反应更快。2) 对于限制性饮食者而言,目标和分心物之间的交互作用显著,事后检验发现,当目标刺激为高能量图片时,限制性饮食者对高能量的分心刺激(HHH)比低能量的分心刺激(LHL)反应更慢;当目标刺激为低能量图片时,限制性饮食者对高能量的分心刺激(HLH)比低能量的分心刺激(LLL)反应更慢。3) 高能量分心物对限制性饮食者增强的干扰与知觉到的节食成功性成显著负相关。结论:限制性饮食者对高能量分心刺激的抵抗力更弱,伴随着这种较弱的抵抗力,其知觉到的节食上的自我调节成功性也更低。
Abstract: Purposes: The research examined the self-regulation ability of Restrained eaters (REs) and unrestrained eaters (UREs) by the resistance to the distractor interference. Methods: 87 female participants (45REs, 42UREs) were involved. We chose the flanker task which was programmed using E-Prime 2.0 on a 17-inch monitor, which were consisted of the target stimulus and flanker stimulus, and the participants were told to pay their attention to the target stimulus only, ignoring the flanker stimulus, the flanker task involving high caloric food-cues and low caloric food-cues pictures. Results: 1) For the UREs, there was a main effect of targets; they responded faster to the high calorie targets overall. 2) For the REs, the analysis revealed a target × flanker interaction. Post hoc t-tests revealed that when the target was high caloric pictures, female REs responded slower when presented with high (HHH) than low calorie flanker pictures (LHL); when the target was low caloric pictures, female REs responded slower when presented with high (HLH) than low calorie flanker pictures (LLL). 3) Furthermore, this increased distraction was inversely associated with Perceived Self-Regulatory Success in Dieting (PSRS). Conclusion: These results indicate that REs showed lower resistance to high caloric distractor interference, and along with the weak resistance, the perception of Self-Regulatory Success in Dieting is lower too.
文章引用:王维, 陈红, 周一舟, 王密 (2017). 限制性饮食者的自我调节能力. 心理学进展, 7(11), 1381-1388. https://doi.org/10.12677/AP.2017.711171

参考文献

[1] 孔繁昌, 张妍, 陈红, 石明丽, Jackson, T., 高笑(2011). 限制性饮食者对食物线索的认知偏向: 行为和脑机制的证据. 心理科学进展, 19(9), 1355-1362.
[2] Ashleigh, H., Eva, K., Robyn, M., & Philip, M. (2014). Resisting Temptation of Unhealthy Food: Interaction between Temptation-Elicited Goal Activation and Self-Control. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 485-495.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9393-6
[3] Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
[4] Fedoroff, I. D. C., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1997). The Effect of Pre-Exposure to Food Cues on the Eating Behavior of Re-Strained and Unrestrained Eaters. Appetite, 28, 33-47.
https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1996.0057
[5] Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us Not unto Temptation. Momentary Allurements Elicit Overriding Goal Activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296-309.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296
[6] Forestell, C. A., Lau, P., Gyurovski, I. I., Dickter, C. L., & Haque, S. S. (2012). Attentional Biases to Foods: The Effects of Caloric Content and Cognitive Restraint. Appetite, 59, 748-754.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.07.006
[7] Friedman, M. I., Ulrich, P., & Mattes, R. D. (1999). A Figurative Measure of Subjective Hunger Sensations. Appetite, 32, 395-404.
https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1999.0230
[8] Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and Unrestrained Eating. Journal of Personality, 43, 647-660.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00727.x
[9] Jansen, A., & van den Hout, H. (1991). On Being Led into Temptation: “Counter Regulation” of Dieters after Smelling a “Preload”. Addictive Behaviors, 16, 247-253.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(91)90017-C
[10] Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, A. M., Samuels, B., & Chatman, J. (2007). Medicare’s Search for Effective Obesity Treatments. Diets Are Not the Answer. American Psychologist, 62, 220-233.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.220
[11] Meule, A., Vögele, C., & Kübler, A. (2012).Restrained Eating Is Related to Accelerated Reaction to High Caloric Foods and Cardiac Autonomic Dysregulation. Appetite, 58, 638-644.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.023
[12] Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The Allure of Forbidden Food. On the Role of Attention in Self-Regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1283-1292.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.008
[13] Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The Neural Basis of Drug Craving. An Incentive-Sensitization Theory of Addiction. Brain Research reviews, 18, 247-291.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P
[14] Roefs, A., Hermanb, C. P., MacLeodc, C. M., Smuldersa, F. T. Y., & Jansena, A. (2005). At First Sight: How Do Restrained Eaters Evaluate High-Fat Palatable Foods? Appetite, 44, 103-114.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.001
[15] Stroebe, W., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Papie, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2013). Why Most Dieters Fail but Some Succeed: A Goal Conflict Model of Eating Behavior. Psychological Review, 120, 110-138.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030849
[16] Veenstra, E. M., de Jong, P. J., Koster, E. H. W., & Roefs, A. (2010). Attentional Avoidance of High-Fat Food in Unsuccessful Dieters. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 282-288.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.02.006