愤怒的加工机制—从效价到动机
The Process Mechanism of Anger——From Valence to Motivation
DOI: 10.12677/AP.2012.23022, PDF, HTML, XML,  被引量 下载: 3,457  浏览: 17,643  国家自然科学基金支持
作者: 荆秀娟*, 王荣燕*:西南大学心理学院;王一峰*:电子科技大学生命科学与技术学院;李 红*:辽宁师范大学心理发展与教育研究中心
关键词: 愤怒效价动机偏侧化情境Anger; Valence; Motivation; Lateralization; Situation
摘要: 愤怒是一种由挑衅刺激诱发的情绪。情绪的效价假说和动机假说认为愤怒是一种有趋近动机倾向的负性情绪。然而,最近的行为和神经成像研究都对这两种假说提出了质疑,研究者用愤怒表达和特异性等观点来解释这些新发现,但这些解释仍有局限性。情景化的动机模型可能更好地解释愤怒在一般情境和特殊情境下的加工机制,即愤怒是由真实情境或经验诱发的,与动机和行为具有一致性的,对挑衅性刺激的反应。
Abstract: Anger is a kind of emotion caused by provocative stimuli. According to the valence hypothesis and the motivation hypothesis, anger is a negative emotion prone to approach motivation. Recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies, however, challenged these hypotheses. Some researchers explained these new findings in opinions of anger expression or anger specificity. These opinions were efficient but limited. A contextually dependent view of motivation model might be adopted to better understand the mechanism of anger both in general situation and in special situation. It regarded anger as the reaction to provocative stimuli, which was induced by real situation or experience and was in conformity with motivation and behavior.
文章引用:荆秀娟, 王一峰, 王荣燕, 李红 (2012). 愤怒的加工机制—从效价到动机. 心理学进展, 2(3), 134-140. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/AP.2012.23022

参考文献

[1] 邹吉林, 张小聪, 张环, 于靓, 周仁来(2011). 超越效价和唤醒——情绪的动机维度模型述评. 心理科学进展, 9期, 1339-1346.
[2] Adolphs, R., Russell, J. A., & Tranel, D. (1999). A role for the human amygdala in recognizing emotional arousal from unpleasant stimuli. Psychological Science, 10, 167-171.
[3] Balconi, M., & Mazza, G. (2010). Lateralisation effect in comprehension of emotional facial expression: A comparison between EEG alpha band power and behavioural inhibition (BIS) and activation (BAS) systems. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 15, 361-384.
[4] Bassett, D. S., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2011). Understanding complexity in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 200-209.
[5] Beaver, J. D., Lawrence, A. D., Passamonti, L., & Calder, A. J. (2008). Appetitive motivation predicts the neural response to facial signals of aggression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 2719-2725.
[6] Borod, J. C., Zgaljardic, D., Tabert, M. H., & Koff, E. (2001). Asymmetries of emotional perception and expression in normal adults. In G. Gainotti (Ed.), Handbookof neuropsychology (Vol. 5, pp. 181- 205). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
[7] Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., & Young, A. W. (2001). Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 352-363.
[8] Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 321-352.
[9] Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-re- lated affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183-204.
[10] Cattaneo, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2009). The mirror neuron system. Archives of Neurology, 66, 557-560.
[11] Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., Nogawa, J., Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Sadato, N., & Ambady, N. (2008). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2167-2174.
[12] Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). The association of anger and hostility with future coronary heart disease: A meta-analytic review of pros-pective evidence. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 53, 936-946.
[13] Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology, 40, 106-114.
[14] Davidson, R. J. (1984). Affect, cognition and hemispheric specialization. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition and behavior (pp. 320-365). New York: Cambridge University Press.
[15] Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 11-21.
[16] Davidson, R. J., Shackman, A. J., & Maxwell, J. S. (2004). Asymmetries in face and brain related to emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 389-391.
[17] Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish, & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45-60). Chichester: Wiley.
[18] Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164, 86-88.
[19] Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 111-116.
[20] Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804-818.
[21] Fetterman, A. K., Robinson, M. D., Gordon, R. D., & Elliot, A. J. (2011). Anger as seeing red: Perceptual sources of evidence. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 311-316.
[22] Forgas, J. P., & George, J. M. (2001). Affective influences on judgments and behavior in organizations: An information processing perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 3-34.
[23] Hareli, S., & Rafaeli, A. (2008). Emotion cycles: On the social influence of emotion in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 28, 35-39.
[24] Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310-1316.
[25] Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emo-tion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84, 451-462.
[26] Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2011). Leaning embodies desire: Evidence that leaning forward increases relative left frontal cortical activation to appetitive stimuli. Biological Psychology, 87, 311-313.
[27] Harmon-Jones, E., & Peterson, C. K. (2009). Supine body position reduces neural response to anger evocation. Psychological Science, 20, 1209-1210.
[28] Harmon-Jones, E., Peterson, C. K., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2010). Anger, motivation, and asymmetrical frontal cortical activations. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger: Constituent and concomitant biological, psychological, and social processes (pp. 61-78). New York: Springer.
[29] Herrington, J. D., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Engels, A. S., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Localization of asymmetric brain function in emotion and depression. Psychophysiology, 47, 442-454.
[30] Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 653-670.
[31] Jack, R. E., Blais, C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G., & Caldara, R. (2009). Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal. Current Biology, 19, 1543-1548.
[32] Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, in press.
[33] Kalin, N. H., Larson, C., Shelton, S. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1998). Asymmetric frontal brain activity, cortisol, and behavior associated with fearful temperament in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 286-2892.
[34] Kim, D.-Y., & Lee, J.-H. (2011). Effects of the BAS and BIS on deci-sion-making in a gambling task. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1131-1135.
[35] Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 173, 1-86.
[36] MacLean, P. D. (1993). Cerebral evolution of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 67-83). New York: Guilford.
[37] Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal “accents” cultural differences in facial expres-sions of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 373-376.
[38] Martin, R., Watson, D., & Wan, C. K. (2000). A three-factor model of trait anger: Dimensions of affect, behavior, and cognition. Journal of Personality, 68, 869-897.
[39] Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & Veerdonk, E. V. D. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365-381.
[40] Mayan, I., & Meiran, N. (2011). Anger and the speed of full-body approach and avoidance reactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1-7.
[41] Mendoza, S. P., & Ruys, J. D. (2001). The beginning of an alternative view of the neurobiology of emotion. Social Science Information, 40, 39-60.
[42] Meunier, D., Lambiotte, R., & Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 200.
[43] Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 207-233.
[44] Nash, K., McGregor, I., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation. Psychophysiology, 47, 979- 983.
[45] Newman, M. G., & Llera, S. J. (2011). A novel theory of experiential avoidance in generalized anxiety disorder: A review and synthesis of research supporting a contrast avoidance model of worry. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 371-382.
[46] Pizzagalli, D. A., Sherwood, R. J., Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Frontal brain asymmetry and reward responsiveness: A source-localization study. Psychological Science, 16, 805-813.
[47] Price, T. F., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Approach motivational body postures lean toward left frontal brain activity. Psychophysiology, 48, 718-722.
[48] Price, T. F., Peterson, C. K., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). The emotive neuroscience of embodiment. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 27-37.
[49] Reuter, M., Weber, B., Fiebach, C. J., Elger, C., & Montag, C. (2009). The biological basis of anger: Associations with the gene coding for DARPP-32 (PPP1R1B) and with amygdala volume. Behavioural Brain Research, 202, 179-183.
[50] Robinson, R. G., & Manes, F. (2000). Elation, mania, and mood disorders: Evidence from neurological disease. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion (pp. 239-268). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[51] Rohlfs, P., & Ramírez, M. (2006). Aggression and brain asymmetries: A theoretical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 283-297.
[52] Rutherford, H. J. V., & Lindell, A. K. (2011). Thriving and surviving: Approach and avoidance motivation and lateralization. Emotion Review, 3, 333-343.
[53] Sackeim, H. A., Gur, R. C., & Saucy, M. C. (1978). Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science, 202, 434-436.
[54] Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., & Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and control of anger. In H. Kassinove (Ed.), Anger disorders: Definition, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 49-67). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
[55] Stewart, J. L., Silton, R. L., Sass, S. M., Fisher, J. E., Edgar, C., Heller, W., & Miller, G. A. (2010). Attentional bias to negative emotion as a function of approach and withdrawal anger styles: An ERP investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76, 9-18.
[56] Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843-850.
[57] Taggart, P., Boyett, M. R., Logantha, S., & Lambiase, P. D. (2011). Anger, emotion, and arrhythmias: From brain to heart. Frontiers in Physiology, 2, 67.
[58] Tomarken, A. J., & Zald, D. H. (2009). Conceptual, methodological, and empirical ambiguities in the linkage between anger and approach: Comment on Carver and Harmon-Jones. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 209-214.
[59] Videbeck, S. L. (2010). Psychiatric-mental health nursing (4 Edition). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
[60] Watson, D. (2009). Locating anger in the hierarchical structure of affect: Comment on Carver and Harmon-Jones. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 205-208.
[61] Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2012). When aggressive individuals see the world more accurately: The case of perceptual sensitivity to subtle facial expressions of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 540-553.
[62] Zinner, L. R., Brodish, A. B., Devine, P. G., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Anger and asymmetrical frontal cortical activity: Evidence for an anger—Withdrawal relationship. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 1081-1093.