[1]
|
曹胜强(2019). 跨文化交际中的中西方人际关系认知差异. 现代交际, (11), 55-56.
|
[2]
|
崔梓芊, 张环, 刘希平(2025). “说出即相信”效应: 基于共享现实的解释. 心理发展与教育, 41(1), 145-152.
|
[3]
|
丁莹, 郑全全(2011). “说出即相信”效应的影响因素和理论解释. 心理科学进展, 19(12), 1851-1858.
|
[4]
|
何晓英(2022). 社会互动情境中文化符号的注意偏向及其干预. 硕士学位论文, 银川: 宁夏大学.
|
[5]
|
胡娜, 桑标, 刘俊升, 潘婷婷, 黄元元(2020). 社会互动视角下的人际情绪调节: 概念、测量和理论取向. 心理科学, 43(5), 1227-1234.
|
[6]
|
卢雪(2022). 社会建构论对心理学理论发展的影响. 开封文化艺术职业学院学报, 42(12), 118-120.
|
[7]
|
吕行, 钟年(2016). 性别刻板印象的人际传播特点研究——基于社会认同理论或社会支配理论? 心理科学, 39(1), 185-190.
|
[8]
|
孙亚茹, 刘泽军, 段亚杰, 陈宁, 刘伟(2023). 协作如何减少记忆错误: 一项元分析研究. 心理学报, 55(11), 1780-1795.
|
[9]
|
王国猛, 孙吴信宜, 郑全全, 赵曙明(2016). 情绪创造力对员工创新行为的影响: 情绪社会建构理论的视角. 心理科学, 39(1), 124-130.
|
[10]
|
王潇, 李文忠, 杜建刚(2010). 情绪感染理论研究述评. 心理科学进展, 18(8), 1236-1245.
|
[11]
|
臧鸿瑜(2024). 共同身份认同与次级身份认同对群际关系发展的影响. 硕士学位论文, 兰州: 西北师范大学.
|
[12]
|
Abel, M., & Bäuml, K. T. (2020). Social Interactions Can Simultaneously Enhance and Distort Memories: Evidence from a Collaborative Recognition Task. Cognition, 200, Article 104254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104254
|
[13]
|
Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., & Fox, E. B. (2012). Learning and Remembering with Others: The Key Role of Retrieval in Shaping Group Recall and Collective Memory. Social Cognition, 30, 121-132. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2012.30.1.121
|
[14]
|
Blumen, H. M., & Rajaram, S. (2009). Effects of Repeated Collaborative Retrieval on Individual Memory Vary as a Function of Recall versus Recognition Tasks. Memory, 17, 840-846. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210903266931
|
[15]
|
Browning, C. A., Harris, C. B., & Van Bergen, P. (2019). Successful and Unsuccessful Collaborative Processes in Strangers and Couples Performing Prospective Memory Tasks. Discourse Processes, 56, 655-674. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2018.1541398
|
[16]
|
Browning, C. A., Harris, C. B., Van Bergen, P., Barnier, A. J., & Rendell, P. G. (2018). Collaboration and Prospective Memory: Comparing Nominal and Collaborative Group Performance in Strangers and Couples. Memory, 26, 1206-1219. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1433215
|
[17]
|
Cardoso, C., Valkanas, H., Serravalle, L., & Ellenbogen, M. A. (2016). Oxytocin and Social Context Moderate Social Support Seeking in Women during Negative Memory Recall. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 70, 63-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.001
|
[18]
|
Conte, N., Pelegrina, S., Padulo, C., Marascia, E., & Fairfield, B. (2024). Collaborative Encoding with a New Categorization Task: A Contribution to Collaborative Memory Research. Psychological Research, 88, 1339-1351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01929-w
|
[19]
|
Cuc, A., Koppel, J., & Hirst, W. (2007). Silence Is Not Golden: A Case for Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting. Psychological Science, 18, 727-733. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01967.x
|
[20]
|
de la Mata, M. L., Santamaría, A., & Ruiz, L. (2010). Cultura y memoria autobiográfica: Concepciones del yo en estudiantes universitarios mexicanos y españoles. Estudios de Psicología, 31, 21-38. https://doi.org/10.1174/021093910790744491
|
[21]
|
de la Mata, M. L., Santamaria, A., Ruiz, L., & Hansen, T. G. B. (2011). Autobiographical Memory, Narrative and Self-Construal. A Cross-Cultural Study. Revista Mexicana de Psicologia, 28, 183-191.
|
[22]
|
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why Faces Are and Are Not Special: An Effect of Expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 107-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.107
|
[23]
|
Doherty, B. R., Patai, E. Z., Duta, M., Nobre, A. C., & Scerif, G. (2017). The Functional Consequences of Social Distraction: Attention and Memory for Complex Scenes. Cognition, 158, 215-223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.015
|
[24]
|
Eskenazi, T., Doerrfeld, A., Logan, G. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2013). Your Words Are My Words: Effects of Acting Together on Encoding. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1026-1034. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.725058
|
[25]
|
Greimel, E., Nehrkorn, B., Fink, G. R., Kukolja, J., Kohls, G., Müller, K. et al. (2012). Neural Mechanisms of Encoding Social and Non-Social Context Information in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3440-3449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.029
|
[26]
|
Griffiths, K., Stretton, J., & Dalgleish, T. (2022). Memory Bias for Social Hierarchical Information Is Modulated by Perceived Social Rank. Memory, 30, 650-657. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2022.2029902
|
[27]
|
Guan, L., & Wang, Q. (2022). Does Sharing Memories Make Us Feel Closer? The Roles of Memory Type and Culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 53, 344-361. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221211072809
|
[28]
|
Gutchess, A., & Cho, I. (2024). Memory and Aging across Cultures. Current Opinion in Psychology, 55, Article 101728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101728
|
[29]
|
Hadar, B., Luria, R., & Liberman, N. (2020). Induced Social Power Improves Visual Working Memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46, 285-297. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219855045
|
[30]
|
Harris, C. B., Barnier, A. J., Sutton, J., & Khan, T. (2017). Social Contagion of Autobiographical Memories. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6, 319-327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.07.006
|
[31]
|
Hirst, W., & Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in Conversations: The Social Sharing and Reshaping of Memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 55-79. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100340
|
[32]
|
Hong, M. K., Polyn, S. M., & Fazio, L. K. (2019). Examining the Episodic Context Account: Does Retrieval Practice Enhance Memory for Context? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 4, Article No. 46. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-019-0202-3
|
[33]
|
Kelley, M. R., Reysen, M. B., Ahlstrand, K. M., & Pentz, C. J. (2012). Collaborative Inhibition Persists Following Social Processing. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 727-734. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.684945
|
[34]
|
Kensinger, E. A., Choi, H., Murray, B. D., & Rajaram, S. (2016). How Social Interactions Affect Emotional Memory Accuracy: Evidence from Collaborative Retrieval and Social Contagion Paradigms. Memory & Cognition, 44, 706-716. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0597-8
|
[35]
|
Knausenberger, J., Wagner, U., Higgins, E. T., & Echterhoff, G. (2019). Epistemic Authority in Communication Effects on Memory: Creating Shared Reality with Experts on the Topic. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8, 439-449. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101840
|
[36]
|
Leger, K. R., & Gutchess, A. (2021). Cross-Cultural Differences in Memory Specificity: Investigation of Candidate Mechanisms. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10, 33-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.08.016
|
[37]
|
Meade, M. L., Nokes, T. J., & Morrow, D. G. (2009). Expertise Promotes Facilitation on a Collaborative Memory Task. Memory, 17, 39-48. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802524240
|
[38]
|
Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., Sturkie, C. M., Rochester, N. M., Liu, X., & Gutchess, A. H. (2018). Cross-Cultural Differences in Item and Background Memory: Examining the Influence of Emotional Intensity and Scene Congruency. Memory, 26, 751-758. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1406119
|
[39]
|
Nemeth, D., Turcsik, A. B., Farkas, G., & Janacsek, K. (2013). Social Communication Impairs Working-Memory Performance. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 20, 211-214. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2012.685134
|
[40]
|
Nie, A., Ke, C., Guo, B., Li, M., & Xiao, Y. (2021). Collaborative Memory for Categorized Lists: Ongoing and Lasting Effects Are Sensitive to Episodic Memory Tasks. Current Psychology, 42, 3870-3887. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01684-w
|
[41]
|
Nie, A., Li, M., Li, M., Xiao, Y., & Wang, S. (2022). Together We Lose or Gain: Ongoing and Enduring Impacts of Collaboration in Episodic Memory of Emotional DRM Lists. Current Psychology, 42, 27965-27982. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03940-z
|
[42]
|
Orianne, J., & Eustache, F. (2023). Collective Memory: Between Individual Systems of Consciousness and Social Systems. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article 1238272. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1238272
|
[43]
|
Parker, A., Dagnall, N., & Coyle, A. (2007). Environmental Context Effects in Conceptual Explicit and Implicit Memory. Memory, 15, 423-434. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701309834
|
[44]
|
Parker, A., Gellatly, A., & Waterman, M. (1999). The Effect of Environmental Context Manipulation on Memory: Dissociation between Perceptual and Conceptual Implicit Tests. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 555-570. https://doi.org/10.1080/095414499382291
|
[45]
|
Richardson, D. C., Street, C. N. H., Tan, J. Y. M., Kirkham, N. Z., Hoover, M. A., & Ghane Cavanaugh, A. (2012). Joint Perception: Gaze and Social Context. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, Article 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00194
|
[46]
|
Rossi-Arnaud, C., Spataro, P., Bhatia, D., Doricchi, F., Mastroberardino, S., & Cestari, V. (2020). Long-Lasting Positive Effects of Collaborative Remembering on False Assents to Misleading Questions. Acta Psychologica, 203, Article 102986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102986
|
[47]
|
Saraiva, M., Albuquerque, P. B., & Garrido, M. V. (2023). Collaborative Inhibition Effect: The Role of Memory Task and Retrieval Method. Psychological Research, 87, 2548-2558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01821-z
|
[48]
|
Schwartz, A. J., Boduroglu, A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2014). Cross‐Cultural Differences in Categorical Memory Errors. Cognitive Science, 38, 997-1007. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12109
|
[49]
|
Shteynberg, G. (2010). A Silent Emergence of Culture: The Social Tuning Effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 683-689. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019573
|
[50]
|
Wagner, U., & Echterhoff, G. (2018). When Does Oxytocin Affect Human Memory Encoding? The Role of Social Context and Individual Attachment Style. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, Article 349. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00349
|
[51]
|
Wagner, U., Giesen, A., Knausenberger, J., & Echterhoff, G. (2017). The Joint Action Effect on Memory as a Social Phenomenon: The Role of Cued Attention and Psychological Distance. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1697. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01697
|
[52]
|
Wagner, U., Schlechter, P., & Echterhoff, G. (2022). Socially Induced False Memories in the Absence of Misinformation. Scientific Reports, 12, Article No. 7725. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11749-w
|
[53]
|
Walter, S., & Meier, B. (2017). Social Importance Enhances Prospective Memory: Evidence from an Event-Based Task. Memory, 25, 777-783. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1221973
|
[54]
|
Wong, S. F., Cardoso, C., Orlando, M. A., Brown, C. A., & Ellenbogen, M. A. (2022). Depressive Symptoms and Social Context Modulate Oxytocin’s Effect on Negative Memory Recall. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 1234-1243. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab072
|
[55]
|
Ye, S., Ye, T., Duan, Z., & Ding, X. (2023). Working Memory for Gaze Benefits from the Face Context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 31, 1516-1526. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02430-y
|
[56]
|
Zhang, H., Liu, Y., Wang, X., Cui, Z., Wang, H., Liu, X. et al. (2023). Benefits of Collaborative Remembering in Older and Younger Couples: The Role of Conversation Dynamics and Gender. Memory, 31, 406-420. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2023.2166963
|