[1]
|
毕鸿燕, 方格(2001). 4-6岁幼儿空间方位传递性推理能力的发展. 心理学报, 3期, 238-243.
|
[2]
|
毕鸿燕, 方格, 翁旭初(2004). 小学儿童两维空间方位传递性推理能力的发展. 心理学报, 2期, 174-178.
|
[3]
|
李红, 林崇德(2001). 个体解决三项系列问题的心理模型. 心理学报, 6期, 518-525.
|
[4]
|
莫秀峰, 李红, 张仲明(2011). 3~5岁幼儿在视野阻隔任务中的长度传递性推理. 心理发展与教育, 3期, 225-232.
|
[5]
|
魏勇刚, 尹荣, 庞丽娟(2010). 传递性推理与儿童早期非标准测量的关系. 心理科学, 2期, 449-451.
|
[6]
|
杨娟, 邱江, 张庆林(2008). 传递性推理的认知与脑机制探讨. 心理科学, 3期, 663-666.
|
[7]
|
赵雷(2005). 工作记忆和传递性推理. 武汉: 华中科技大学硕士论文, 未出版.
|
[8]
|
赵雷, 周治金, 刘昌(2006). 工作记忆成分在传递性推理中的作用. 心理科学, 5期, 1058-1062.
|
[9]
|
张仲明, 李红(2007). 个体解决5项系列问题的复合模型研究. 心理科学, 1期, 104-107.
|
[10]
|
张凤华, 邱江, 杨群, 张庆林(2009). 传递性推理的ERP研究. 心理发展与教育, 4期, 68-74.
|
[11]
|
Acuna, B.D., Eliassen, J.C., Donoghue, J.P., & Sanes, J.N. (2002). Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 1312-1321.
|
[12]
|
Brigitte, M.W., Sophia, K., & Christian, S. (2010). Transitive inference in free-living greylag geese, Anser anser. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1277-1283.
|
[13]
|
Delis, D.C., Squire, L.R., Bihrle, A., & Massman, P. (1992). Componential analysis of problem-solving ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting test. Neuropsychologia, 30, 683-697.
|
[14]
|
Denise, M.W., & Rebecca, L.G. (2013). Generalizing memories over time: Sleep and reinforcement facilitate transitive inference. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 100, 70-76.
|
[15]
|
Dickins, D.W, Singh, K.D, Roberts, N., Burns, P., Downes, J.J., Jimmieson, P., et al. (2001). An fMRI study of stimulus equivalence. NeuroReport, 12, 405-411.
|
[16]
|
Demarais, A.M., & Cohen, B.H. (1998). Evidence for image-scanning eye movements during transitive inference. Biological Psychology, 49, 229-247.
|
[17]
|
Dusek, J.A., & Eichenbaum, H. (1997). The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, 7109-7114.
|
[18]
|
Eichenbaum, H. (2004). Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron, 44, 109-120.
|
[19]
|
Goel, V., Buchel, C., Frith, C., & Dolan, R.J. (2000). Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllogistic reasoning. NeuroImage, 12, 504-514.
|
[20]
|
Goel, V., & Dolan, R.J. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of three-term relational reasoning. Neuropsychologia, 39, 901-909.
|
[21]
|
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 293-302.
|
[22]
|
Heckers, S., Zalesak, M., Weiss, A.P., Ditman, T., & Titone, D. (2004). Hippocampal activation during transitive inference in humans. Hippocampus, 14, 153-162.
|
[23]
|
Jennifer, D.R., Sandra, N.M., & Christina, V. (2009). Impaired relational organization of propositions, but intact transitive inference, in aging: Implications for understanding underlying neural integrity. Neuropsychologia, 47, 338-353.
|
[24]
|
Johnson-Laird, P.N., Oakhill, J., & Bull, D. (1986). Children’s syllogistic reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 35-58.
|
[25]
|
Martin, Z., & Stephan, H. (2009). The role of the hippocampus in transitive inference. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 172, 24-30.
|
[26]
|
Nagode, J.C., & Pardo, J.V. (2002). Human hippocampal activation during transitive inference. Neuroreport, 13, 939-944.
|
[27]
|
Olga, F.L., & Edward, A.W. (2012). Transitive inference in pigeons: Measuring the associative values of Stimuli B and D. Behavioural Processes, 89, 244-255.
|
[28]
|
Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Rivière, D., & LebiHan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage, 14, 1013-1026.
|
[29]
|
Randall, L.W., Vinod, G., Vanessa, R., Frank, K., & Jordan, G. (2013). Transitive inference reasoning is impaired by focal lesions in parietal cortex rather than rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 51, 464-471.
|
[30]
|
Rickard, T.C., Romero, S.G., Wharton, C., Flitman, S., & Grafman, J. (2000). The calculating brain: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 38, 325-335.
|
[31]
|
Sternberg, R.J. (1980). The development of linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 342356.
|
[32]
|
Titone, D., Ditman, T., Holzman, P.S., Eichenbaum, H., & Levy, D.L. (2004). Transitive inference in schizophrenia: Impairments in relational memory organization. Schizophrenia Research, 68, 235-247.
|
[33]
|
Waltz, J.A., Knowlton, B.J., Holyoak, K.J., Boone, K.B., Mishkin, F.S., de Menezes Santos, M., et al. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10, 119-125.
|
[34]
|
Wharton, C.M., Grafman, J., Flitman, S.S., Hansen, E.K., Brauner, J., Marks, A., et al. (2000). Toward neuroanatomical models of analogy: A positron emission tomography study of analogical mapping. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 173-197.
|
[35]
|
Wynne, C.D.L., & Staddo, J.E.R. (1998). A minimal model of transitive inference. Models of action: Mechanisms for adaptive behavior. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 269-307.
|
[36]
|
Yonelinas, A.P., Kroll, N.E., Quamme, J.R., et al. (2002). Effects of extensive temporal lobe damage or mild hypoxia on recollection and familiarity. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1236-1241.
|
[37]
|
Zentall, T.R., & Sherburne, L.M. (1994). Transfer of value from s+ to sin a simultaneous discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 176-183.
|