[1]
|
Garner, R., Gillingham, M.G. and White, C.S. (1989) Effects of ‘Seductive Details’ on Macroprocessing and Microprocessing in Adults and Children. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 41-57. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0601_2
|
[2]
|
Harp, S.F. and Mayer, R.E. (1998) How Seductive Details Do Their Damage: A Theory of Cognitive Interest in Science Learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414-434. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-0663.90.3.414
|
[3]
|
Harp, S.F. and Mayer, R.E. (1997) The Role of Interest in Learning from Scientific Text and Illustrations: On the Distinction between Emotional Interest and Cognitive Interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 92-102. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.92
|
[4]
|
Lehman, S., Schraw, G., McCrudden, M.T. and Hartley, K. (2007) Processing and Recall of Seductive Details in Scientific Text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 569-587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.07.002
|
[5]
|
Rey, G.D. (2012) A Review of Research and a Meta-Analysis of the Seductive Detail Effect. Educational Research Review, 7, 216-237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2012.05.003
|
[6]
|
Mayer, R.E. and Fiorella, L. (2021) Introduction to Multimedia Learning. In: Mayer, R.E. and Fiorella, L., Eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, Cambridge University Press, 3-16. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894333.003
|
[7]
|
Rey, G.D. (2014) Seductive Details and Attention Distraction—An Eye Tracker Experiment. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 133-144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.11.017
|
[8]
|
Sundararajan, N. and Adesope, O. (2020) Keep It Coherent: A Meta-Analysis of the Seductive Details Effect. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 707-734. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09522-4
|
[9]
|
Mayer, R.E. (2019) Taking a New Look at Seductive Details. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 139-141. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3503
|
[10]
|
Hidi, S. and Baird, W. (1988) Strategies for Increasing Text-Based Interest and Students’ Recall of Expository Texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 465-483. https://doi.org/10.2307/747644
|
[11]
|
Ketzer‐Nöltge, A., Schweppe, J. and Rummer, R. (2018) Is the Seductive Details Effect Moderated by Mood? An Eye‐tracking Study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 62-70. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3487
|
[12]
|
Sweller, J. (1988) Cognitive Load during Problem Solving: Effects on Learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257-285. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1202_4
|
[13]
|
Sweller, J. (1994) Cognitive Load Theory, Learning Difficulty, and Instructional Design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295-312. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5
|
[14]
|
Mayer, R.E. (2005) The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning. Cambridge University Press.
|
[15]
|
Mayer, R.E. and Moreno, R. (1998) A Split-Attention Effect in Multimedia Learning: Evidence for Dual Processing Systems in Working Memory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 312-320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.312
|
[16]
|
Mayer, R.E. and Moreno, R. (2003) Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43-52. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3801_6
|
[17]
|
Korbach, A., Brünken, R. and Park, B. (2016) Learner Characteristics and Information Processing in Multimedia Learning: A Moderated Mediation of the Seductive Details Effect. Learning and Individual Differences, 51, 59-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.08.030
|
[18]
|
Sanchez, C.A. and Wiley, J. (2006) An Examination of the Seductive Details Effect in Terms of Working Memory Capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34, 344-355. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193412
|
[19]
|
Mayer, R.E., Griffith, E., Jurkowitz, I.T.N. and Rothman, D. (2008) Increased Interestingness of Extraneous Details in a Multimedia Science Presentation Leads to Decreased Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 329-339. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013835
|
[20]
|
Eitel, A., Bender, L. and Renkl, A. (2018) Are Seductive Details Seductive Only When You Think They Are Relevant? An Experimental Test of the Moderating Role of Perceived Relevance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 20-30. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3479
|
[21]
|
Eitel, A. and Kühl, T. (2019) Editorial: Harmful or Helpful to Learning? The Impact of Seductive Details on Learning and Instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 3-8. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3513
|
[22]
|
Strobel, B., Grund, S. and Lindner, M.A. (2018) Do Seductive Details Do Their Damage in the Context of Graph Comprehension? Insights from Eye Movements. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 95-108. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3491
|
[23]
|
Tsai, M., Wu, A. and Chen, Y. (2019) Static and Dynamic Seductive Illustration Effects on Text‐and‐Graphic Learning Processes, Perceptions, and Outcomes: Evidence from Eye Tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 109-123. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3514
|
[24]
|
Schneider, S., Wirzberger, M. and Rey, G.D. (2018) The Moderating Role of Arousal on the Seductive Detail Effect in a Multimedia Learning Setting. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 71-84. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3473
|
[25]
|
Abercrombie, S., Hushman, C.J. and Carbonneau, K.J. (2018) The Impact of Seductive Details and Signaling on Analogical Transfer. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33, 38-47. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3475
|
[26]
|
Koriat, A. and Bjork, R.A. (2005) Illusions of Competence in Monitoring One’s Knowledge during Study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 187-194. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.187
|
[27]
|
Alter, A.L., Oppenheimer, D.M., Epley, N. and Eyre, R.N. (2007) Overcoming Intuition: Metacognitive Difficulty Activates Analytic Reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569-576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569
|
[28]
|
Finn, B. and Tauber, S.K. (2015) When Confidence Is Not a Signal of Knowing: How Students’ Experiences and Beliefs about Processing Fluency Can Lead to Miscalibrated Confidence. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 567-586. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9313-7
|
[29]
|
Rothkopf, E.Z. and Billington, M.J. (1979) Goal-Guided Learning from Text: Inferring a Descriptive Processing Model from Inspection Times and Eye Movements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 310-327. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.71.3.310
|
[30]
|
Maloy, J., Fries, L., Laski, F. and Ramirez, G. (2019) Seductive Details in the Flipped Classroom: The Impact of Interesting but Educationally Irrelevant Information on Student Learning and Motivation. CBE—Life Sciences Education, 18, ar42. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.19-01-0004
|
[31]
|
Alexander, P.A. (2003) The Development of Expertise: The Journey from Acclimation to Proficiency. Educational Researcher, 32, 10-14. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189x032008010
|
[32]
|
Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T. and Brünken, R. (2011) Does Cognitive Load Moderate the Seductive Details Effect? A Multimedia Study. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 5-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.006
|
[33]
|
Wang, Z., Ardasheva, Y. and Lin, L. (2019) Does High Perceptual Load Assist in Reducing the Seductive Details Effect? Educational Psychology, 41, 25-44. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2019.1686465
|
[34]
|
Wang, Z. and Adesope, O. (2017) Do Focused Self-Explanation Prompts Overcome Seductive Details? A Multimedia Study. Educational Technology & Society, 20, 47-57.
|
[35]
|
Abercrombie, S. (2013) Transfer Effects of Adding Seductive Details to Case-Based Instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 149-157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.01.002
|
[36]
|
Peshkam, A., Mensink, M.C., Putnam, A.L. and Rapp, D.N. (2011) Warning Readers to Avoid Irrelevant Information: When Being Vague Might Be Valuable. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 219-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.006
|