[1]
|
陈琳, 钟罗金, 李悠, 莫雷(2014). 特征呈现方式对类别知觉表征的影响. 心理与行为研究, 12(5), 577-582.
|
[2]
|
冯霞, 冯成志(2022). 认知灵活性对概率类别学习的影响. 心理学报, 54(11), 1340-1353.
|
[3]
|
刘万伦, 张奇(2008). 类别学习的神经心理学研究. 心理科学进展, 16(1), 36-41.
|
[4]
|
刘万伦, 张奇(2010). 类别数量对基于规则和信息整合类别学习的影响. 心理科学, 33(5), 1230-1233.
|
[5]
|
刘志雅, 莫雷(2006). 类别学习中两种学习模式的比较研究: 分类学习与推理学习. 心理学报, 38(6), 38-46.
|
[6]
|
孙海龙, 邢强(2014). 反馈对知觉类别学习的影响及其认知神经生理机制. 心理科学进展, 22(1), 67-74.
|
[7]
|
吴洁, 付秋芳, 周晓燕, 孙洵伟(2018). 特征的呈现方式对类别学习中规则和相似性知识获得的影响. 心理科学, 41(5), 1031-1038.
|
[8]
|
邢强, 孙海龙, 车敬上(2014). 观察学习与反馈学习方式对类别学习的影响. 广州大学学报(社会科学版), 13(12), 17-22.
|
[9]
|
余玉荣(2020). 监测的呈现方式对类别学习及CLJ的影响. 现代交际, (11), 185-186.
|
[10]
|
余玉荣, 刘便荣, 邢强(2022). 样例多样性和流畅性信念对类别学习判断的影响. 心理学探新, 42(6), 499-507.
|
[11]
|
岳芳, 陈剑平, 甘可鑫, 王宇情, 刘志雅(2023). 交叉重叠类别结构的自主学习优势和集中学习劣势. 心理学报, 55(10), 1597-1607.
|
[12]
|
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the Operation of an Explicit Rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3-19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.120.1.3
|
[13]
|
Ashby, F. G., & Gott, R. E. (1988). Decision Rules in the Perception and Categorization of Multidimensional Stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 33-53. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.14.1.33
|
[14]
|
Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (1992). Complex Decision Rules in Categorization: Contrasting Novice and Experienced Performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 50-71. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.18.1.50
|
[15]
|
Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (1993). Relations between Prototype, Exemplar, and Decision Bound Models of Categorization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 372-400. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmps.1993.1023
|
[16]
|
Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). Human Category Learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 149-178. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070217
|
[17]
|
Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2011). Human Category Learning 2.0. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224, 147-161. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05874.x
|
[18]
|
Bharani, K. L., Paller, K. A., Reber, P. J., Weintraub, S., Yanar, J., & Morrison, R. G. (2016). Compensatory Processing during Rule-Based Category Learning in Older Adults. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 23, 304-326.
|
[19]
|
Birnbaum, M. S., Kornell, N., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). Why Interleaving Enhances Inductive Learning: The Roles of Discrimination and Retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41, 392-402. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0272-7
|
[20]
|
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A Study of Thinking. John Wiley and Sons.
|
[21]
|
Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Effects of Interleaved and Blocked Study on Delayed Test of Category Learning Generalization. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 936. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00936
|
[22]
|
Casale, M. B., & Ashby, F. G. (2008). A Role for the Perceptual Representation Memory System in Category Learning. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 983-999. https://doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.6.983
|
[23]
|
Daehler, M. W., Lonardo, R., & Bukatko, D. (1979). Matching and Equivalence Judgments in Very Young Children. Child Development, 50, 170-179. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129053
|
[24]
|
Dobson, J. L. (2011). Effect of Selected “Desirable Difficulty” Learning Strategies on the Retention of Physiology Information. Advances in Physiology Education, 35, 378-383. https://doi.org/10.1152/advan.00039.2011
|
[25]
|
Erickson, M. A., & Kruschke, J. K. (1998). Rules and Exemplars in Category Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 107-140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.2.107
|
[26]
|
Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2004). Multidimensional Rule, Unidimensional Rule, and Similarity Strategies in Categorization: Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1026-1044. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.5.1026
|
[27]
|
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Wiley.
|
[28]
|
Grimm, L. R., & Maddox, W. T. (2013). Differential Impact of Relevant and Irrelevant Dimension Primes on Rule-Based and Information-Integration Category Learning. Acta Psychologica, 144, 530-537.
|
[29]
|
Higgins, E., & Ross, B. (2011). Comparisons in Category Learning: How Best to Compare for What. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 33, 1388-1393.
|
[30]
|
Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Learning Painting Styles: Spacing Is Advantageous When It Promotes Discriminative Contrast. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 97-103. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1801
|
[31]
|
Kéri, S. (2003). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Category Learning. Brain Research Reviews, 43, 85-109. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00204-2
|
[32]
|
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning Concepts and Categories: Is Spacing the “Enemy of Induction”? Psychological Science, 19, 585-592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x
|
[33]
|
Kornell, N., Castel, A. D., Eich, T. S., & Bjork, R. A. (2010). Spacing as the Friend of Both Memory and Induction in Young and Older Adults. Psychology and Aging, 25, 498-503. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017807
|
[34]
|
Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context Theory of Classification Learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207-238. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.85.3.207
|
[35]
|
Metcalfe, J., & Xu, J. (2016). People Mind Wander More during Massed than Spaced Inductive Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 978-984. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000216
|
[36]
|
Minda, J. P., & Smith, J. D. (2002). Prototypes in Category Learning: The Effects of Category Size, Category Structure, and Stimulus Complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 775-799.
|
[37]
|
Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a Silk Purse Out of Two Sow’s Ears: Young Children’s Use of Comparison in Category Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 5-15. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.5
|
[38]
|
Nomura, E., Maddox, W., Filoteo, J., Ing, A., Gitelman, D., Parrish, T. et al. (2007). Neural Correlates of Rule-Based and Information-Integration Visual Category Learning. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 37-43. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj122
|
[39]
|
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, Similarity, and the Identification-Categorization Relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.39
|
[40]
|
Nosofsky, R. M., Palmeri, T. J., & McKinley, S. C. (1994). Rule-Plus-Exception Model of Classification Learning. Psychological Review, 101, 53-79. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.53
|
[41]
|
Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern Recognition and Categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 382-407. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90014-x
|
[42]
|
Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The Shuffling of Mathematics Problems Improves Learning. Instructional Science, 35, 481-498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9015-8
|
[43]
|
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9
|
[44]
|
Seger, C. A., & Miller, E. K. (2010). Category Learning in the Brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 203-219. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135546
|
[45]
|
Smith, J. D., Beran, M. J., Crossley, M. J., Boomer, J., & Ashby, F. G. (2010). Implicit and Explicit Category Learning by Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) and Humans (Homo Sapiens). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 54-65. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015892
|
[46]
|
Sorensen, L. J., & Woltz, D. J. (2016). Blocking as a Friend of Induction in Verbal Category Learning. Memory & Cognition, 44, 1000-1013. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0615-x
|
[47]
|
Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2002). The Effect of Category Variability in Perceptual Categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 893-907. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.5.893
|
[48]
|
Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., & Bouwmeester, S. (2014). Is Spacing Really the “Friend of Induction”? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 259. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00259
|
[49]
|
Vincent, A. M., Russell, J. W., Low, P., & Feldman, E. L. (2004). Oxidative Stress in the Pathogenesis of Diabetic Neuropathy. Endocrine Reviews, 25, 612-628. https://doi.org/10.1210/er.2003-0019
|
[50]
|
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations. Blackwells.
|
[51]
|
Yamauchi, T., & Markman, A. B. (1988). Category Learning by Inference and Classification. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 124-148. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2566
|
[52]
|
Zeithamova, D. (2008). Category Learning Systems. PhD Thesis, The University of Texas at Austin.
|
[53]
|
Zulkiply, N. (2015). The Role of Bottom-Up vs. Top-Down Learning on the Interleaving Effect in Category Induction. Pertanika Journal of Social Science & Humanities, 23, 933-944.
|
[54]
|
Zulkiply, N., & Burt, J. S. (2013). The Exemplar Interleaving Effect in Inductive Learning: Moderation by the Difficulty of Category Discriminations. Memory & Cognition, 41, 16-27. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0238-9
|