[1]
|
蒋毅, 王莉(2011). 生物运动加工特异性: 整体结构和局部运动的作用. 心理科学进展, 19(3), 301-311.
|
[2]
|
Adolphs, R. (1999). Social Cognition and the Human Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469-479. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01399-6
|
[3]
|
Ahmed, A. A., & Vander Wyk, B. C. (2013). Neural Processing of Intentional Biological Motion in Unaffected Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An fMRI Study. Brain and Cognition, 83, 297-306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.007
|
[4]
|
Alaerts, K., Nackaerts, E., Van Gemert, T., Durnez, M., Mestdagh, A., Meyns, P. et al. (2010). Action and Emotion Recognition from Point Light Displays: An Investigation of Gender Differences in Functions Mediated by the Human Mirror system. Society for Neuroscience.
|
[5]
|
Alaerts, K., Woolley, D. G., Steyaert, J., Di Martino, A., Swinnen, S. P., & Wenderoth, N. (2014). Underconnectivity of the Superior Temporal Sulcus Predicts Emotion Recognition Deficits in Autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1589-1600. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst156
|
[6]
|
Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired Recognition of Emotions from Body Movements Is Associated with Elevated Motion Coherence Thresholds in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3023-3029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.019
|
[7]
|
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body Cues, Not Facial Expressions, Discriminate between Intense Positive and Negative Emotions. Science, 338, 1225-1229. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1224313
|
[8]
|
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in Emotion Perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 286-290. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422522
|
[9]
|
Becker, D. V., Anderson, U. S., Mortensen, C. R., Neufeld, S. L., & Neel, R. (2011). The Face in the Crowd Effect Unconfounded: Happy Faces, Not Angry Faces, Are More Efficiently Detected in Single-and Multiple-Target Visual Search Tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 637-659. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024060
|
[10]
|
Bertenthal, B. I. (1993). Infants’ Perception of Biomechanical Motions: Intrinsic Image and Knowledge-Based Constraints. Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy, 21, 175-214.
|
[11]
|
Chang, D. H. F., & Troje, N. F. (2008). Perception of Animacy and Direction from Local Biological Motion Signals. Journal of Vision, 8, 3. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.3
|
[12]
|
Cheng, Y., Yuan, X., & Jiang, Y. (2023). Eye Pupil Signals Life Motion Perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 86, 579-586. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02729-x
|
[13]
|
Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The Visual Analysis of Emotional Actions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 63-74. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600630599
|
[14]
|
Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The Perception of Emotion from Body Movement in Point-Light Displays of Interpersonal Dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171-1180. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5203
|
[15]
|
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, Plasticity, Context, and Regulation: Perspectives from Affective Neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 890-909. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.890
|
[16]
|
De Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the Neurobiology of Emotional Body Language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 242-249. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1872
|
[17]
|
Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (1999). Neural Mechanisms Subserving the Perception of Human Actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 172-178. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01312-1
|
[18]
|
Dittrich, W. H. (1993). Action Categories and the Perception of Biological Motion. Perception, 22, 15-22. https://doi.org/10.1068/p220015
|
[19]
|
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the Universality and Cultural Specificity of Emotion Recognition: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203
|
[20]
|
Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Häberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W. et al. (2008). Perception of Biological Motion in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1480-1494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.025
|
[21]
|
Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2002). Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion. Neuron, 35, 1167-1175. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00897-8
|
[22]
|
Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G. et al. (2000). Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 711-720. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562417
|
[23]
|
Jiang, Y., & He, S. (2006). Cortical Responses to Invisible Faces: Dissociating Subsystems for Facial-Information Processing. Current Biology, 16, 2023-2029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.084
|
[24]
|
Jiang, Y., Shannon, R. W., Vizueta, N., Bernat, E. M., Patrick, C. J., & He, S. (2009). Dynamics of Processing Invisible Faces in the Brain: Automatic Neural Encoding of Facial Expression Information. NeuroImage, 44, 1171-1177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.038
|
[25]
|
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual Perception of Biological Motion and a Model for Its Analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 201-211. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03212378
|
[26]
|
Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self Recognition versus Recognition of Others by Biological Motion: Viewpoint-Dependent Effects. Perception, 35, 911-920. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5540
|
[27]
|
Kaiser, M. D., Delmolino, L., Tanaka, J. W., & Shiffrar, M. (2010). Comparison of Visual Sensitivity to Human and Object Motion in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Research, 3, 191-195. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.137
|
[28]
|
Killgore, W. D. S., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2004). Activation of the Amygdala and Anterior Cingulate during Nonconscious Processing of Sad versus Happy Faces. NeuroImage, 21, 1215-1223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.033
|
[29]
|
Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2008). Altered Face Scanning and Impaired Recognition of Biological Motion in a 15‐Month‐Old Infant with Autism. Developmental Science, 11, 40-46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00608.x
|
[30]
|
Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2011). Neural Correlates of Coherent and Biological Motion Perception in Autism. Developmental Science, 14, 1075-1088. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01058.x
|
[31]
|
Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the Sex of a Walker from a Dynamic Point-Light Display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575-580. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03198740
|
[32]
|
Lee, H., & Kim, J. (2017). Facilitating Effects of Emotion on the Perception of Biological Motion: Evidence for a Happiness Superiority Effect. Perception, 46, 679-697. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616681809
|
[33]
|
Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing People from Their Movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210-220. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.210
|
[34]
|
Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring Intentions from Biological Motion: A Stimulus Set of Point-Light Communicative Interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 168-178. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.42.1.168
|
[35]
|
Mazzoni, N. (2017). The Recognition of Emotional Biological Movement in Individuals with Typical Development and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Doctoral Dissertation, University of Trento.
|
[36]
|
Missana, M., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Infants’ Emerging Sensitivity to Emotional Body Expressions: Insights from Asymmetrical Frontal Brain Activity. Developmental Psychology, 51, 151-160. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038469
|
[37]
|
Missana, M., Atkinson, A. P., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Tuning the Developing Brain to Emotional Body Expressions. Developmental Science, 18, 243-253. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12209
|
[38]
|
Nackaerts, E., Wagemans, J., Helsen, W., Swinnen, S. P., Wenderoth, N., & Alaerts, K. (2012). Recognizing Biological Motion and Emotions from Point-Light Displays in Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLOS ONE, 7, e44473. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044473
|
[39]
|
Norman, J. F., Payton, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging and the Perception of Biological Motion. Psychology and Aging, 19, 219-225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.219
|
[40]
|
Pavlova, M. A. (2012). Biological Motion Processing as a Hallmark of Social Cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 981-995. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr156
|
[41]
|
Sun, Y., Wang, X., Huang, Y., Ji, H., & Ding, X. (2022). Biological Motion Gains Preferential Access to Awareness during Continuous Flash Suppression: Local Biological Motion Matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151, 309-320. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001078
|
[42]
|
Swettenham, J., Remington, A., Laing, K., Fletcher, R., Coleman, M., & Gomez, J. (2013). Perception of Pointing from Biological Motion Point-Light Displays in Typically Developing Children and Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1437-1446. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1699-1
|
[43]
|
Troje, N. F. (2002). Decomposing Biological Motion: A Framework for Analysis and Synthesis of Human Gait Patterns. Journal of Vision, 2, 2. https://doi.org/10.1167/2.5.2
|
[44]
|
Troje, N. F. (2008). Retrieving Information from Human Movement Patterns. In T. F. Shipley, & J. M. Zacks (Eds.), Understanding Events: From Perception to Action (pp. 308-334). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195188370.003.0014
|
[45]
|
Vanrie, J., & Verfaillie, K. (2004). Perception of Biological Motion: A Stimulus Set of Human Point-Light Actions. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 625-629. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206542
|
[46]
|
Wang, L., Zhang, K., He, S., & Jiang, Y. (2010). Searching for Life Motion Signals. Psychological Science, 21, 1083-1089. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610376072
|
[47]
|
Whalen, P.J., Kagan, J., Cook, R.G., Davis, F.C., Kim, H., Polis, S., et al. (2004) Human Amygdala Responsivity to Masked Fearful Eye Whites. Science, 306, 2061-2061. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103617
|
[48]
|
Williams, M. A., Morris, A. P., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., & Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2898-2904. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4977-03.2004
|
[49]
|
Yuan, T., Ji, H., Wang, L., & Jiang, Y. (2023). Happy Is Stronger than Sad: Emotional Information Modulates Social Attention. Emotion, 23, 1061-1074. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001145
|