[1]
|
林崇德. 发展心理学[M]. 北京: 人民教育出版社, 2018.
|
[2]
|
马伟娜, 洪灵敏, 桑标. 同伴交往、亲子交往与儿童心理理论发展的关系[J]. 心理科学, 2009, 32(1): 81-84.
|
[3]
|
刘明, 邓赐平, 桑标. 幼儿心理理论与社会行为发展关系的初步研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2002, 18(2): 39-42.
|
[4]
|
Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978) Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515-526. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00076512
|
[5]
|
Perner, J. and Wimmer, H. (1985) “John Thinks That Mary Thinks That…” Attribution of Second-Order Beliefs by 5 to 10-Year-Old Children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437-471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7
|
[6]
|
Wellman, H.M., Cross, D. and Watson, J. (2001) Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth about False Belief. Child Development, 72, 655-684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304
|
[7]
|
Wellman, H.M. (2011) Developing a Theory of Mind. In: Goswami, U., Ed., The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, Blackwell Publication, 258-284.
|
[8]
|
Wellman, H.M. and Liu, D. (2004) Scaling of Theory-of-Mind Tasks. Child Development, 75, 523-541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
|
[9]
|
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y. and Plumb, I. (2001) The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241-251. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00715
|
[10]
|
Boccadoro, S., Cracco, E., Hudson, A.R., Bardi, L., Nijhof, A.D., Wiersema, J.R., et al. (2019) Defining the Neural Correlates of Spontaneous Theory of Mind (tom): An Fmri Multi-Study Investigation. NeuroImage, 203, 116193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116193
|
[11]
|
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F. and Perner, J. (2014) Fractionating Theory of Mind: A Meta-Analysis of Functional Brain Imaging Studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009
|
[12]
|
Schurz, M., Maliske, L. and Kanske, P. (2020) Cross-Network Interactions in Social Cognition: A Review of Findings on Task Related Brain Activation and Connectivity. Cortex, 130, 142-157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.006
|
[13]
|
Liu, D., Sabbagh, M.A., Gehring, W.J. and Wellman, H.M. (2009) Neural Correlates of Children’s Theory of Mind Development. Child Development, 80, 318-326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01262.x
|
[14]
|
Kleiman-Weiner, M., Vientós, A., Rand, D.G. and Tenenbaum, J.B. (2025) Evolving General Cooperation with a Bayesian Theory of Mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 122, e2400993122. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2400993122
|
[15]
|
王美芳, 陈会昌. 错误信念理解后儿童心理理论的发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2001, 17(2): 45-49.
|
[16]
|
Clements, W.A. and Perner, J. (1994) Implicit Understanding of Belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377-395. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
|
[17]
|
Onishi, K.H. and Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? Science, 308, 255-258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621
|
[18]
|
黄琪钰, 刘秀丽. 婴儿是否拥有错误信念理解能力? [J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(3): 431-442.
|
[19]
|
Wellman, H.M. and Woolley, J.D. (1990) From Simple Desires to Ordinary Beliefs: The Early Development of Everyday Psychology. Cognition, 35, 245-275. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-e
|
[20]
|
Saylor, M.M., Baldwin, D.A., Baird, J.A. and LaBounty, J. (2007) Infants’ On-Line Segmentation of Dynamic Human Action. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 113-128. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370709336996
|
[21]
|
Astington, J.W. (2001) The Paradox of Intention: Assessing Children’s Meta-Representational Understanding. In: Malle, B., Moses, L. and Baldwin, D., Eds., Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition, The MIT Press, 85-104. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3838.003.0008
|
[22]
|
Liao, Y., Li, H. and Deák, G.O. (2011) Can Unpredicted Outcomes Be Intended? the Role of Outcome-Beliefs in Children's Judgments of Intention. Cognitive Development, 26, 106-117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.003
|
[23]
|
Colonnesi, C., Zijlstra, B.J.H., van der Zande, A. and Bögels, S.M. (2012) Coordination of Gaze, Facial Expressions and Vocalizations of Early Infant Communication with Mother and Father. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 523-532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.02.004
|
[24]
|
Jenkins, J.M. and Astington, J.W. (1996) Cognitive Factors and Family Structure Associated with Theory of Mind Development in Young Children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 70-78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.1.70
|
[25]
|
Milligan, K., Astington, J.W. and Dack, L.A. (2007) Language and Theory of Mind: Meta‐analysis of the Relation between Language Ability and False‐Belief Understanding. Child Development, 78, 622-646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
|
[26]
|
赵婧, 苏彦捷. 学龄儿童的情景记忆与心理理论的关系[J]. 心理与行为研究, 2013, 11(2): 158-163.
|
[27]
|
童薇, 阳泽, 张霞. 幼儿心理理论与时序记忆的关系——来自时序记忆分离的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2012, 28(3): 248-254.
|
[28]
|
Wheeler, M.A., Stuss, D.T. and Tulving, E. (1997) Toward a Theory of Episodic Memory: The Frontal Lobes and Autonoetic Consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.331
|
[29]
|
Müller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D.P., Carpendale, J.I.M., Hammond, S.I. and Bibok, M.B. (2012) Knowing Minds, Controlling Actions: The Developmental Relations between Theory of Mind and Executive Function from 2 to 4 Years of Age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 331-348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.014
|
[30]
|
Moses, L.J. (2001) Executive Accounts of Theory-of-Mind Development. Child Development, 72, 688-690. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00306
|
[31]
|
Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J. and Claxton, L.J. (2004) Individual Differences in Executive Functioning and Theory of Mind: An Investigation of Inhibitory Control and Planning Ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299-319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002
|
[32]
|
魏勇刚, 吴睿明, 李红, 冯廷勇, Philip, D. 抑制性控制在幼儿执行功能与心理理论中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(5): 598-605.
|
[33]
|
Ruffman, T., Perner, J. and Parkin, L. (1999) How Parenting Style Affects False Belief Understanding. Social Development, 8, 395-411. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00103
|
[34]
|
Perner, J. and Leekam, R.S.R. (1994) Theory of Mind Is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228-1238.
|
[35]
|
Shatz, M., Diesendruck, G., Martinez-Beck, I. and Akar, D. (2003) The Influence of Language and Socioeconomic Status on Children’s Understanding of False Belief. Developmental Psychology, 39, 717-729. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.717
|
[36]
|
Peterson, C.C., Wellman, H.M. and Liu, D. (2005) Steps in Theory-of-Mind Development for Children with Deafness or Autism. Child Development, 76, 502-517. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x
|
[37]
|
Wright, B.C. and Mahfoud, J. (2011) A Child-Centred Exploration of the Relevance of Family and Friends to Theory of Mind Development. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 32-40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00920.x
|
[38]
|
Slaughter, V. and Gopnik, A. (2010) Conceptual Coherence in the Child’s Theory of Mind: Training Children to Understand Belief. Child Development, 67, 2967-2988.
|
[39]
|
Vinden, P.G. (2001) Parenting Attitudes and Children’s Understanding of Mind: A Comparison of Korean American and Anglo-American Families. Cognitive Development, 16, 225-245.
|
[40]
|
崔云. 3-6岁城市与农村儿童心理理论发展及其家庭养育环境的对比实验研究[D]: [硕士学位论文]. 重庆: 西南师范大学, 2002.
|
[41]
|
Ames, D.R. (2004) Strategies for Social Inference: A Similarity Contingency Model of Projection and Stereotyping in Attribute Prevalence Estimates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 573-585. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.573
|
[42]
|
Rizzolatti, G. and Craighero, L. (2004) The Mirror-Neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
|
[43]
|
Gopnik, A. and Wellman, H.M. (2012) Reconstructing Constructivism: Causal Models, Bayesian Learning Mechanisms, and the Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085-1108. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028044
|
[44]
|
崔苗, 徐西英, 郑玉玮. 心理理论的发展: 来自发展心理学和社会神经科学的证据[J]. 心理技术与应用, 2016, 4(11): 684-695.
|
[45]
|
Mahy, C.E.V., Moses, L.J. and Pfeifer, J.H. (2014) How and Where: Theory-of-Mind in the Brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 68-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002
|
[46]
|
Leslie, A.M., Friedman, O. and German, T.P. (2004) Core Mechanisms in ‘Theory of Mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 528-533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001
|
[47]
|
Young, L., Camprodon, J.A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A. and Saxe, R. (2010) Disruption of the Right Temporoparietal Junction with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Reduces the Role of Beliefs in Moral Judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 6753-6758. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914826107
|
[48]
|
Rothmayr, C., Sodian, B., Hajak, G., Döhnel, K., Meinhardt, J. and Sommer, M. (2010) Common and Distinct Neural Networks for False-Belief Reasoning and Inhibitory Control. NeuroImage, 56, 1705-1713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.052
|
[49]
|
Sabbagh, M.A. and Taylor, M. (2000) Neural Correlates of Theory-of-Mind Reasoning: An Event-Related Potential Study. Psychological Science, 11, 46-50. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00213
|
[50]
|
Liu, D., Sabbagh, M.A., Gehring, W.J. and Wellman, H.M. (2009) Neural Correlates of Children’s Theory of Mind Development. Child Development, 80, 318-326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01262.x
|
[51]
|
Meinhardt, J., Kühn-Popp, N., Sommer, M. and Sodian, B. (2012) Distinct Neural Correlates Underlying Pretense and False Belief Reasoning: Evidence from ERPs. NeuroImage, 63, 623-631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.019
|
[52]
|
蒋钦. 心理理论的认知神经基础[D]: [博士学位论文]. 沈阳: 辽宁师范大学, 2018.
|
[53]
|
Bowman, L.C., Liu, D., Meltzoff, A.N. and Wellman, H.M. (2012) Neural Correlates of Belief and Desire‐Reasoning in 7 and 8‐Year-Old Children: An Event-Related Potential Study. Developmental Science, 15, 618-632. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01158.x
|
[54]
|
Wang, Y.W., Huang, L., Lin, C.D., et al. (2012) Spatio-Temporal Brain Dynamics of Understanding Social versus Private Intentions: An Electrical Neuroimaging Study. Neuro Quantology, 10, 733-743. https://doi.org/10.14704/nq.2012.10.4.608
|
[55]
|
Leuthold, H., Filik, R., Murphy, K. and Mackenzie, I.G. (2012) The On-Line Processing of Socio-Emotional Information in Prototypical Scenarios: Inferences from Brain Potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 457-466. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr029
|
[56]
|
Saxe, R. and Kanwisher, N. (2003) People Thinking about Thinking People: The Role of the Temporo-Parietal Junction in “Theory of Mind”. NeuroImage, 19, 1835-1842. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00230-1
|
[57]
|
Schuwerk, T., Döhnel, K., Sodian, B., Keck, I.R., Rupprecht, R. and Sommer, M. (2013) Functional Activity and Effective Connectivity of the Posterior Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Processing of Incongruent Mental States. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 2950-2965. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22377
|
[58]
|
Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. and Decety, J. (2000) A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task. NeuroImage, 11, 157-166. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0525
|
[59]
|
Jackson, P., Melzoff, A.N. and Detecy, J. (2005) How Do We Perceive the Pain of Others? A Window into Neural Processes Involved in Empathy. NeuroImage, 24, 771-779.
|
[60]
|
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Cohen, D.J. (2000) Understanding the Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford Press, 336-337.
|
[61]
|
Stuss, D.T. (2001) The Frontal Lobes Are Necessary for ‘Theory of Mind’. Brain, 124, 279-286. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.2.279
|
[62]
|
Fine, C., Lumsden, J. and Blair, R.J.R. (2001) Dissociation between ‘Theory of Mind’ and Executive Functions in a Patient with Early Left Amygdala Damage. Brain, 124, 287-298. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.2.287
|
[63]
|
Stone, V.E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J. and Young, A. (2003) Acquired Theory of Mind Impairments in Individuals with Bilateral Amygdala Lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 209-220. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00151-3
|