[1]
|
卞迁, 齐薇, 刘志方, 闫国利(2009). 当代眼动记录技术述评. 心理研究, 2(1), 34-37.
|
[2]
|
陈庆荣, 谭顶良, 邓铸, 周临, 张晓丽(2009). 眼跳的研究范式及其主要认知功能. 心理科学进展, 17(6), 1197-1210.
|
[3]
|
陈庆荣, 周曦, 韩静, 安静(2012). 眼球追踪: 模式、技术和应用. 实验室研究与探索, 31(10), 10-15.
|
[4]
|
陈婷婷, 蒋长好, 丁锦红(2012). 视觉运动追踪的加工过程. 心理科学进展, 20(3), 354-364.
|
[5]
|
窦东徽, 石敏, 赵然, 刘肖岑(2014). 社会生态心理学: 探究个体与环境关系的新取向. 北京师范大学学报: 社会科学版, (5), 43-54.
|
[6]
|
高世欢, 陈顺森, 苏彦捷, 林彩云(2019). 视觉正常的自闭症儿童双眼注视点间距的特点及其意义. 心理学报, 51(9), 1018-1027.
|
[7]
|
胡金生, 李骋诗, 王琦, 李松泽, 李涛涛, 刘淑清(2018). 孤独症青少年的情绪韵律注意偏向缺陷: 低效率的知觉模式. 心理学报, 50(6), 637-646.
|
[8]
|
季红光, 郭迪, 蔡汝刚, 王海明(2001). 婴幼儿习惯化行为的研究概况. 中国行为医学科学, 10(4), 397-398.
|
[9]
|
李琳, 赵赛男, 张俐娟, 王敬欣(2022). 老年人汉语阅读中预测误差成本的产生机制. 心理科学进展, 30(1), 1-14.
|
[10]
|
李莎, 栗觅, 吕胜富, 付冰冰, 丰雷, 王刚, 钟宁(2016). 视觉搜索范式下抑郁症患者的负性注意偏向. 中国心理卫生杂志, 30(3), 167-171.
|
[11]
|
李珊珊, 尤冉, 郭笑霄, 赵露, 王艳玲, 陈曦(2021). 视神经再生机制研究与进展. 中国组织工程研究, 25(23), 3740-3745.
|
[12]
|
李文清, 张珊珊, 徐琨鹏(2020). 眼动仪在认知行为实验中的应用及选择. 软件导刊, 19(2), 80-83.
|
[13]
|
李月(2018). 毕生发展心理学的新进展——评《毕生发展心理学(第二版)》. 中国教育学刊, (11), 142.
|
[14]
|
林崇德(1994). 从儿童心理学到发展心理学. 北京师范大学学报: 社会科学版, (1), 1-7.
|
[15]
|
林崇德(2019). 中国的发展心理学七十年. 心理发展与教育, 35(5), 632-640.
|
[16]
|
林崇德, 陈英和(2009). 中国发展心理学30年的进展. 北京师范大学学报: 社会科学版, (1), 38-46.
|
[17]
|
刘静怡, 黄希庭, 杨帅(2013). 社交焦虑障碍中的注意偏差三成分研究述评. 心理科学进展, 21(4), 664-670.
|
[18]
|
刘燊, 甘烨彤, 汪琛, 王高峰(2022). 中国发展心理学的研究热点与发展趋势——基于《心理发展与教育》创刊35年的文献计量分析. 心理发展与教育, 38(3), 447-456.
|
[19]
|
刘志方, 仝文, 张智君, 王永胜(2021). 中文阅读中切词过程的年老化: 眼动证据. 心理学探新, 41(1), 34-40.
|
[20]
|
陆润豪, 张兴利, 施建农(2021). 眼动技术在个体认知能力差异研究中的应用. 心理科学, 44(3), 552-558.
|
[21]
|
马恒芬, 张长颉(2023). 基于眼动追踪技术的认知语言学研究进展. 中国生物医学工程学报, 42(6), 750-756.
|
[22]
|
齐亚菲, 梁良, 莫书亮, 王福兴(2016). 孤独症儿童对限制性兴趣刺激的视觉注意: 一项眼动研究. 中国特殊教育, (5), 29-34.
|
[23]
|
嵩钰佳, 王菠, 吕正欣(2018). 婴幼儿早期阅读的方法. 黑龙江教育学院学报, 37(6), 80-82.
|
[24]
|
王福兴, 童钰, 钱莹莹, 谢和平(2016). 眼动追踪技术与婴幼儿研究: 程序、方法与数据分析. 心理与行为研究, 14(4), 558-567.
|
[25]
|
夏海莎, 李雨谿, 刘小菠, 范金, 张家铭, 苟馨云, 李涓, 郑重, 金荣疆(2022). 抑郁患者情绪面孔注视特征: 基于眼动研究的Meta分析. 中国组织工程研究, 26(29), 4735-4741.
|
[26]
|
徐娟(2012). 眼动仪的发展和性能比较. 中国现代教育装备, (23), 16-18.
|
[27]
|
许洁, 王豪龙(2020). 阅读行为眼动跟踪研究综述. 出版科学, 28(2), 52-66.
|
[28]
|
闫国利, 巫金根, 胡晏雯, 白学军(2010). 当前阅读的眼动研究范式述评. 心理科学进展, 18(12), 1966-1976.
|
[29]
|
闫国利, 周丽, 张莉(2019). 空白重叠范式对自闭症儿童的早期预测. 心理科学, 42(1), 223-229.
|
[30]
|
颜志强, 王福兴, 苏彦捷(2016). 疼痛面孔注意加工中共情的作用——来自眼动的证据. 心理科学, 39(3), 573-579.
|
[31]
|
杨晓梦, 王福兴, 王燕青, 赵婷婷, 高春颍, 胡祥恩(2022). 瞳孔是心灵的窗口吗?——瞳孔在心理学研究中的应用及测量. 心理科学进展, 28(7), 1029-1041.
|
[32]
|
杨周, 朱嘉雯, 苏琳, 熊明洁(2024). 对疼痛线索的晚期注视偏向预测慢性疼痛的维持: 来自眼动的证据. 心理学报, 56(1), 44-60.
|
[33]
|
易佳琦, 刘志方, 仝文, 陈朝阳(2023). 阅读中词边界影响词汇加工的年老化研究: 眼动证据. 心理科学, 46(4), 779-786.
|
[34]
|
余益兵, 葛明贵(2004). 世纪之交中的中国发展心理学研究的计量学分析. 心理发展与教育, 20(4), 79-83.
|
[35]
|
张苏媛, 兰泽波, 张水, 孟珠, 闫国利(2021). 无关言语对一年级小学生阅读影响的眼动研究. 心理与行为研究, 19(4), 433-438, 446.
|
[36]
|
周临, 邓铸, 陈庆荣(2012). 反向眼跳的实验范式、机制及影响因素. 心理科学, 35(1), 16-23.
|
[37]
|
朱晓倩, 王一伊, 苏彦捷, 曾晓, 颜志强(2021). 儿童共情研究及其测量工具: 回顾与展望. 心理技术与应用, 9(10), 619-628.
|
[38]
|
Ariel, R., & Castel, A. D. (2013). Eyes Wide Open: Enhanced Pupil Dilation When Selectively Studying Important Information. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 337-344. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3744-5
|
[39]
|
Barsznica, Y., Noiret, N., Lambert, B., Monnin, J., De Pinho, C., Hickel, J. et al. (2021). Saccadic Eye Movements in Elderly Depressed Patients with Suicidal Behaviors: An Exploratory Eye-Tracking Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 712347. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712347
|
[40]
|
Blythe, H. I., Liang, F., Zang, C., Wang, J., Yan, G., Bai, X. et al. (2012). Inserting Spaces into Chinese Text Helps Readers to Learn New Words: An Eye Movement Study. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 241-254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.004
|
[41]
|
Bowie, C. R., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and Interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols, 1, 2277-2281. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.390
|
[42]
|
Brignani, D., Bortoletto, M., Miniussi, C., & Maioli, C. (2010). The When and Where of Spatial Storage in Memory-Guided Saccades. NeuroImage, 52, 1611-1620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.039
|
[43]
|
Bucher, A., Voss, A., Spaniol, J., Hische, A., & Sauer, N. (2019). Age Differences in Emotion Perception in a Multiple Target Setting: An Eye-Tracking Study. Emotion, 20, 1423-1434. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000645
|
[44]
|
Carter, B. T., & Luke, S. G. (2020). Best Practices in Eye Tracking Research. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 155, 49-62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.05.010
|
[45]
|
Chakrabarti, B., Haffey, A., Canzano, L., Taylor, C. P., & McSorley, E. (2017). Individual Differences in Responsivity to Social Rewards: Insights from Two Eye-Tracking Tasks. PLOS ONE, 12, e0185146. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185146
|
[46]
|
Chamberlain, L. (2007). Eye Tracking Methodology: Theory and Practice. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 10, 217-220. https://doi.org/10.1108/13522750710740862
|
[47]
|
Cheng, C., Kaldy, Z., & Blaser, E. (2019a). Focused Attention Predicts Visual Working Memory Performance in 13-Month-Old Infants: A Pupillometric Study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 36, Article ID: 100616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100616
|
[48]
|
Cheng, C., Kaldy, Z., & Blaser, E. (2019b). Two-year-olds Succeed at MIT: Multiple Identity Tracking in 20-and 25-Month-Old Infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 187, Article ID: 104649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.06.002
|
[49]
|
Cherici, C., Kuang, X., Poletti, M., & Rucci, M. (2012). Precision of Sustained Fixation in Trained and Untrained Observers. Journal of Vision, 12, 31-31. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.6.31
|
[50]
|
Coe, B. C., & Munoz, D. P. (2017). Mechanisms of Saccade Suppression Revealed in the Anti-Saccade Task. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372, Article ID: 20160192. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0192
|
[51]
|
Cooper, R. M. (1974) The Control of Eye Fixation by the Meaning of Spoken Language: A New Methodology for the Real-time Investigation of Speech Perception, Memory, and Language Processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(74)90005-X
|
[52]
|
Corbetta, D., Guan, Y., & Williams, J. L. (2012). Infant Eye‐tracking in the Context of Goal‐directed Actions. Infancy, 17, 102-125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00093.x
|
[53]
|
Eimer, M. (2014). The Neural Basis of Attentional Control in Visual Search. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 526-535. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.05.005
|
[54]
|
Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual Experience in Infants: Decreased Attention to Familiar Patterns Relative to Novel Ones. Science, 146, 668-670. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.146.3644.668
|
[55]
|
Franchak, J. M., Kretch, K. S., Soska, K. C., Babcock, J. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2010). Head-mounted Eye-Tracking of Infants’ Natural Interactions. In Proceedings of the 2010 Symposium on Eye-Tracking Research & Applications—ETRA’10 (pp. 21). ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.1145/1743666.1743671
|
[56]
|
Fujioka, T., Tsuchiya, K. J., Saito, M., Hirano, Y., Matsuo, M., Kikuchi, M. et al. (2020). Developmental Changes in Attention to Social Information from Childhood to Adolescence in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Comparative Study. Molecular Autism, 11, Article No. 24. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00321-w
|
[57]
|
Fukushima, K., Fukushima, J., Warabi, T., & Barnes, G. R. (2013). Cognitive Processes Involved in Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements: Behavioral Evidence, Neural Substrate and Clinical Correlation. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7, Article 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00004
|
[58]
|
Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2004). Infants’ Evolving Representations of Object Motion during Occlusion: A Longitudinal Study of 6‐ to 12‐Month‐Old Infants. Infancy, 6, 165-184. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0602_2
|
[59]
|
Gredebäck, G., Johnson, S., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Eye Tracking in Infancy Research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903325758
|
[60]
|
Grönqvist, H., Gredebäck, G., & Hofsten, C. V. (2006). Developmental Asymmetries between Horizontal and Vertical Tracking. Vision Research, 46, 1754-1761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.11.007
|
[61]
|
Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and Secondary Saccades to Goals Defined by Instructions. Vision Research, 18, 1279-1296. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(78)90218-3
|
[62]
|
Hikosaka, O., & Wurtz, R. H. (1983). Visual and Oculomotor Functions of Monkey Substantia Nigra Pars Reticulata. III. Memory-Contingent Visual and Saccade Responses. Journal of Neurophysiology, 49, 1268-1284. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1983.49.5.1268
|
[63]
|
Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Infant Cognition: Going Full Factorial with Pupil Dilation. Developmental Science, 12, 670-679. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00805.x
|
[64]
|
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to Eyes Is Present but in Decline in 2–6-Month-Old Infants Later Diagnosed with Autism. Nature, 504, 427-431. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12715
|
[65]
|
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A Theory of Reading: From Eye Fixations to Comprehension. Psychological Review, 87, 329-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.87.4.329
|
[66]
|
Kobayashi, H., & Kohshima, S. (1997). Unique Morphology of the Human Eye. Nature, 387, 767-768. https://doi.org/10.1038/42842
|
[67]
|
Kucharský, Š., Zaharieva, M., Raijmakers, M., & Visser, I. (2024). Habituation, Part II. Rethinking the Habituation Paradigm. Infant and Child Development, 33, e2383. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2383
|
[68]
|
Lai, M., Tsai, M., Yang, F., Hsu, C., Liu, T., Lee, S. W. et al. (2013). A Review of Using Eye-Tracking Technology in Exploring Learning from 2000 to 2012. Educational Research Review, 10, 90-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2013.10.001
|
[69]
|
Li, S., Li, L., Wang, J., McGowan, V. A., & Paterson, K. B. (2018). Effects of Word Length on Eye Guidance Differ for Young and Older Chinese Readers. Psychology and Aging, 33, 685-692. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000258
|
[70]
|
Lisberger, S. G., Morris, E. J., & Tychsen, L. (1987). Visual Motion Processing and Sensory-Motor Integration for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 97-129. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.000525
|
[71]
|
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological Evidence for Parallel and Serial Processing during Visual Search. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 603-617. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211606
|
[72]
|
McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The Span of the Effective Stimulus during a Fixation in Reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 578-586. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03203972
|
[73]
|
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal Processing of Emotional Information in Anxiety and Depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304-311. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.102.2.304
|
[74]
|
Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A Two-Process Theory of Infant Face Recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164-181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.164
|
[75]
|
Müller, N., Riedel, M., Eggert, T., & Straube, A. (1999). Internally and Externally Guided Voluntary Saccades in Unmedicated and Medicated Schizophrenic Patients. Part II. Saccadic Latency, Gain, and Fixation Suppression Errors. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, 7-14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004060050059
|
[76]
|
Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look Away: The Anti-Saccade Task and the Voluntary Control of Eye Movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 218-228. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1345
|
[77]
|
Nakagawa, A., & Sukigara, M. (2019). Attentional Bias Assessed by a Facial Expression Cuing Paradigm in Infants. Scientific Reports, 9, Article No. 68. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-36806-1
|
[78]
|
Nelson, K. E. (1971). Accommodation of Visual Tracking Patterns in Human Infants to Object Movement Patterns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 12, 182-196. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(71)90003-8
|
[79]
|
Nyström, M., Niehorster, D. C., Andersson, R., & Hooge, I. (2021). The Tobii Pro Spectrum: A Useful Tool for Studying Microsaccades? Behavior Research Methods, 53, 335-353. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01430-3
|
[80]
|
Onkhar, V., Dodou, D., & de Winter, J. C. F. (2024). Evaluating the Tobii Pro Glasses 2 and 3 in Static and Dynamic Conditions. Behavior Research Methods, 56, 4221-4238. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02173-7
|
[81]
|
Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., Langston, R. E., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2013). An Oculomotor Continuum from Exploration to Fixation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 6175-6180. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1222715110
|
[82]
|
Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the Role of Salience in the Allocation of Overt Visual Attention. Vision Research, 42, 107-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00250-4
|
[83]
|
Pärnamets, P., Johansson, P., Hall, L., Balkenius, C., Spivey, M. J., & Richardson, D. C. (2015). Biasing Moral Decisions by Exploiting the Dynamics of Eye Gaze. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 4170-4175. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415250112
|
[84]
|
Peng, M., Browne, H., Cahayadi, J., & Cakmak, Y. (2021). Predicting Food Choices Based on Eye-Tracking Data: Comparisons between Real-Life and Virtual Tasks. Appetite, 166, Article ID: 105477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2021.105477
|
[85]
|
Popa, L., Selejan, O., Scott, A., Mureşanu, D. F., Balea, M., & Rafila, A. (2015). Reading Beyond the Glance: Eye Tracking in Neurosciences. Neurological Sciences, 36, 683-688. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-015-2076-6
|
[86]
|
Posner, M. I. (1984). Components of Visual Orienting. Attention and Performance, 10, 531-556.
|
[87]
|
Rahal, R., & Fiedler, S. (2019). Understanding Cognitive and Affective Mechanisms in Social Psychology through Eye-tracking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 85, Article ID: 103842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103842
|
[88]
|
Rayner, K. (1975). The Perceptual Span and Peripheral Cues in Reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90005-5
|
[89]
|
Rayner, K., & Bertera, J. H. (1979). Reading without a Fovea. Science, 206, 468-469. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.504987
|
[90]
|
Rayner, K., Liversedge, S. P., White, S. J., & Vergilino-Perez, D. (2003). Reading Disappearing Text: Cognitive Control of Eye Movements. Psychological Science, 14, 385-389. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.24483
|
[91]
|
Richards, J. E., & Holley, F. B. (1999). Infant Attention and the Development of Smooth Pursuit Tracking. Developmental Psychology, 35, 856-867. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.856
|
[92]
|
Richmond, J. L., Zhao, J. L., & Burns, M. A. (2015). What Goes Where? Eye Tracking Reveals Spatial Relational Memory during Infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 79-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.013
|
[93]
|
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of Components of Displacement-Step Stimuli Upon Latency for Saccadic Eye Movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 1024-1029. https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.57.001024
|
[94]
|
Schoth, D. E., Nunes, V. D., & Liossi, C. (2012). Attentional Bias Towards Pain-Related Information in Chronic Pain; a Meta-Analysis of Visual-Probe Investigations. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 13-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.004
|
[95]
|
Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals. Current Biology, 18, 668-671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059
|
[96]
|
Sereno, S. C., & Rayner, K. (1992). Fast Priming during Eye Fixation in Reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 173-184. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.173
|
[97]
|
Singh, H., & Singh, J. (2012). Human Eye Tracking and Related Issues: A Review. International Journal of Scientific and Re-search Publications, 2, 1-9.
|
[98]
|
Spering, M., & Carrasco, M. (2015). Acting without Seeing: Eye Movements Reveal Visual Processing without Awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 38, 247-258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.02.002
|
[99]
|
Takarae, Y., Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2008). Patterns of Visual Sensory and Sensorimotor Abnormalities in Autism Vary in Relation to History of Early Language Delay. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 980-989. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617708081277
|
[100]
|
Thothathiri, M., & Snedeker, J. (2008). Give and Take: Syntactic Priming during Spoken Language Comprehension. Cognition, 108, 51-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.012
|
[101]
|
Tomalski, P., Marczuk, K., Pisula, E., Malinowska, A., Kawa, R., & Niedźwiecka, A. (2017). Chaotic Home Environment Is Associated with Reduced Infant Processing Speed under High Task Demands. Infant Behavior and Development, 48, 124-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.04.007
|
[102]
|
Veneri, G., Federico, A., & Rufa, A. (2014). Evaluating the Influence of Motor Control on Selective Attention through a Stochastic Model: The Paradigm of Motor Control Dysfunction in Cerebellar Patient. BioMed Research International, 2014, Article ID: 162423. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/162423
|
[103]
|
Vermeir, I., & Roose, G. (2020). Visual Design Cues Impacting Food Choice: A Review and Future Research Agenda. Foods, 9, Article 1495. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9101495
|
[104]
|
Vidal, M., Turner, J., Bulling, A., & Gellersen, H. (2012). Wearable Eye Tracking for Mental Health Monitoring. Computer Communications, 35, 1306-1311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comcom.2011.11.002
|
[105]
|
Wertli, J., Schötzau, A., Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., & Palmowski-Wolfe, A. (2020). Feasibility of Eye Movement Recordings with the SMI Tracking Bar in 10-to 11-Year-Old Children Performing a Reading Task. Klinische Monatsblätter für Augenheilkunde, 237, 510-516. https://doi.org/10.1055/a-1101-9204
|
[106]
|
Yan, Z., Pei, M., & Su, Y. (2017). Children’s Empathy and Their Perception and Evaluation of Facial Pain Expression: An Eye Tracking Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 2284. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02284
|
[107]
|
Yan, Z., Pei, M., & Su, Y. (2018). Physical Cue Influences Children’s Empathy for Pain: The Role of Attention Allocation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2378. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02378
|
[108]
|
Yang, Z., Jackson, T., & Chen, H. (2013). Effects of Chronic Pain and Pain-Related Fear on Orienting and Maintenance of Attention: An Eye Movement Study. The Journal of Pain, 14, 1148-1157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2013.04.017
|
[109]
|
Yang, Z., Jackson, T., Gao, X., & Chen, H. (2012). Identifying Selective Visual Attention Biases Related to Fear of Pain by Tracking Eye Movements within a Dot-Probe Paradigm. Pain, 153, 1742-1748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.011
|
[110]
|
Yantis, S. (1992). Multielement Visual Tracking: Attention and Perceptual Organization. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 295-340. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90010-y
|
[111]
|
Zammarchi, G., & Conversano, C. (2021). Application of Eye Tracking Technology in Medicine: A Bibliometric Analysis. Vision, 5, Article 56. https://doi.org/10.3390/vision5040056
|